Skip to main content

Cognitive–Emotional Development in Infants

  • Chapter
  • 2691 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter offered a quite detailed account of the development of emotions in infants from birth to about 12 months, using the theoretical frameworks both of Jean Piaget and Alan Sroufe. In the work of both authors, it is evident that emotions begin with reactions that still are quite automatic and, in part, based by spontaneous fluctuations of the Central Nervous System. From about the third week on, however, babies begin to orient towards emotion-laden situations more actively, displaying alertness and interest and turning attentively towards interesting social stimulation. Their increasing alertness to and joy in the presence and actions of adults begin to evoke grins, smiles, and cooing, and they begin to turn actively to stimulation in an effort to assimilate it. Reactions become more rapid over the first 12 weeks, and from the age of 4 months, infants engage in genuine laughter with its energetic bursts of sounds. At first laughter is elicited by gentle stimulation only, but gradually, more vigorous stimulation that at first may have caused crying becomes more effective. Laughter is increasingly related to understanding incongruities and understanding of joking on the part of mother.

A very crucial concern of researchers has been to document the great importance of sensitive caretakers in guiding emotional development. The chapter adds ample evidence to show that caretaker insensitivity or even neglects begin to show early and dramatic consequences for emotional development, consequences that may be carried into later developmental stages, including adulthood. These negative consequences are mediated by the interactional patterns between infants and parents/caretakers, interactions which are crucial in building positive feelings and in synchronizing brain activity of the infant and mother. If such well-attuned interactions are missing, the infant is set on a cause of faulty emotion regulation and vulnerability to poorly regulated negative emotions).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    As noted in chapters to come, genuine integration remains an issue throughout the total span of life.

  2. 2.

    As, for example, when one feels pleasure at a thought.

  3. 3.

    A “scheme” refers to an organized set of experiences about some aspect of reality; an organized conceptual structure.

  4. 4.

    Phonation refers to the production of sounds.

  5. 5.

    An excellent presentation of this work can be found on YouTube under the title “New Insights about attachment in rhesus monkeys.”

  6. 6.

    The term intersubjectivity is used to indicate that the self arises inherently in a relation of communication with and mirroring of/by others (see Labouvie-Vief, 1994, 1996).

  7. 7.

    An informative and empassioned presentation by Dr. Gunnar can be found on YouTube by entering “Megan Gunnar, Hennepin University Partnership 2013.”

  8. 8.

    The procedural memory system contains memories of actions stored in an imagistic implicit, visceral-automatic, and nonconscious fashion that in some sense can be compared to Piaget’s “sensorimotor skills.”

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azak, S., Murison, R., Wentzel-Larsen, T., Smith, L., & Gunnar, M. R. (2012). Maternal depression and infant daytime cortisol. Psychoneuroendicronology, 15(3), 345–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1981). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brain Facts (2012). A companion publication to brain facts.org and a primer on the brain and nervous system. Society for Neuroscience, www.BrainFacts.org

  • Brennan, P., Calhoun, K., Walker, E., & Stowe, Z. (2002, April). Maternal psycho- pathology and infant cortisol level. Paper presented at the international conference on infant studies, Toronto, ON.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, V., Cichetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 525–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, M. (1988). Constructive evolution: Origins and development of Piaget’s thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cichetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991). Contribution from the study of high-risk populations to understanding the development of emotion regulation. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 15–48). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. R. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation—a possible prelude to violence. Science, 289, 591–594.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. (1964). Insight and responsibility: Lectures on the ethical implications of psychoanalysis. New York: W.W.Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. (1984). The life cycle completed. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 200–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., & Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1823/1957). The id and the ego. In J. Rickman (Ed.), A general selection from the works of Sigmund Freud (pp. 210–235). Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M. R., Bruce, J., & Grotevant, H. D. (2000). International adoption of institutionally reared children: Research and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 573–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion. London: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hentel, A., Beebe, B., & Jaffe, J. (2000). Maternal depression at 6 weeks associated with infant self-comfort at 4 months. Poster presented at the international conference on infant studies, Brighton, England.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., Jessell, T., & Siegelbaum, S. A. (2012). Principles of neuroscience (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill (German Translation: Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell (T. M.) (1996). (Eds.), Neurowissenschaften. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J., Plotsky, P. M., Nemeroff, C. B., & Charney, O. S. (2000). Effects of early adverse experiences on brain structure and function: Clinical implications. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 778–790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, I. C., & Rosenblum, L. A. (1969). Effects of separation from mother on infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 159, 681–695.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1989). The intergenerational transmission of child abuse. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 129–159). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1994). Psyche and Eros: Mind and gender in the life course. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G. (1996). Emotions, thought, and gender. In C. Matalesta Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of research on emotions. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., & DeVoe, M. R. (1991). Emotion regulation in adulthood and later life: A developmental view. In K. S. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 11, pp. 171–193). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., & Marquez, M. G. (2004). Dynamic integration: Affect optimization and differentiation in development. In D. Dai & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 237–272). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. Z. (2011). The foundations of ethology. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Main, M., Hesse, E., & Kaplan, N. (2005). Predictability of attachment behaviour and representational processes at 1, 6, and 18 years of age: The Berkeley Longitudinal Study. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Grossmann, & E. Waters (Eds.), Attachment from infancy to adulthood (pp. 245–304). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, B. S., & Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 205–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, E., Marcelli, D., & Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expec-tancies for social contingency in 2-month olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Canet, P., Libert, G., & Gérardin, P. (2005). Two-month old infants of depressed mothers show mild, delayed, and persistent change in emotional state after non-contingent interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 418–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2012). The science of neglect: The persistent absence of responsive care disrupts the developing brain. Working Paper 12. http://www.developingchild.harvard.edu

  • Nelson, C. A., Bos, K., Gunnar, M. R., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2011). The neurobiological toll of early human deprivation. In R. B. McCall, M. H. van IJzendoorn, F. Juffer, C. J. Groark, & V. K. Groza (Eds.), Children without permanent parents: Research, practice, and policy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(4), 127–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., & Biven, L. (2012). The archeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotion. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: W.W. Norton (Original work published 1936).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972). Biology and knowledge. Edinburgh, England: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews (T. A. Brown & C. E. Kaegi, Trans.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self (originally published 1994). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-3459-1.

  • Schore, A. N. (1998). Early shame experiences and infant brain development. In G. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame, interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 337–358). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2001a). Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(1–2), 7–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2001b). The effects of early relational trauma on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22(1–2), 201–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and repair of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Hind, G. W. (1990). Right hemisphere dysfunction in nonverbal learning disabilities: Social, academic, and adaptive functioning in adults and children. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 196–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, G., & Grossman, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, 64, 1439–1450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, G., & Grossman, K. (1999). Individual and physiological correlates of attachment disorganization in infancy. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disorganization (pp. 95–124). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stansbury, K., & Gunnar, M. R. (1994). Adrenocortical activity and emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations (Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 59, No. 2/3, pp. 108–134). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suomi, S. J. (1999). Attachment in rhesus monkeys. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver, (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 181–197). New York: Guilford Press, xvii, 925pp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1989, August). Origins and directions for the concept of infant intersubjectivity. SRCD Newsletter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z., & Weinberg, M. K. (1997). Depressed mothers and infants: Failure to form dyadic states of consciousness. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression in child development (pp. 54–81). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulff, D. M. (1997). Psychology of religion: Classic and contemporary (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaidel, D. W., Esiri, M. M., & Beardsworth, E. D. (1998). Observations on the relationship between verbal explicit and implicit memory and density of neurons in the hippocampus. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1049–1062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Labouvie-Vief, G. (2015). Cognitive–Emotional Development in Infants. In: Integrating Emotions and Cognition Throughout the Lifespan. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09822-7_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics