Advertisement

The Evolution of Joint Attention: A Review and Critique

  • Timothy P. RacineEmail author
  • Tyler J. Wereha
  • Olga Vasileva
  • Donna Tafreshi
  • Joseph J. Thompson
Chapter
Part of the Interdisciplinary Evolution Research book series (IDER, volume 1)

Abstract

Joint attention can be defined as the ability to intentionally coordinate an attentional focus on some object or state of affairs with another. This capacity is believed by most theorists to be logically, developmentally, and evolutionarily prior to language and further forms of social cognition tied up with human social communication. However, although there has been a good deal of empirical and theoretical work on joint attention, there has been less attention paid to the evolution of joint attention in its own right. There has also been sustained debate concerning whether other primates can be said to engage in joint attention, which in turn conditions the evolutionary theories that are offered. In this chapter, we define and describe joint attention, discuss the skills it involves, and the extent to which we share these with other animals. Next, we review work that has been done on the evolution of joint attention and related capacities and classify it as a function of its mode of explanation. We then discuss the aforementioned forms of evolutionary explanation in the light of recent evolutionary theories and findings that question adaptationist thinking, and consider the potential relevance of non-adaptationist thinking for theoretical work on the evolution of joint attention.

Keywords

Joint attention Aadaptationist stance Evolutionary psychology Core knowledge Shared intentionality Non-human primates 

References

  1. Akhtar N, Gernsbacher MA (2007) Joint attention and early vocabulary: a critical look. Lang Linguist Compass 1:195–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews K (2012) Do apes read minds? Toward a new folk psychology. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  3. Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z (2010) False-belief understanding in infants. Trends Cogn Sci 14:110–118PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bakeman R, Adamson LB (1984) Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Dev 55:1278–1289CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  6. Barrett HC, Broesch T, Scott R, He Z, Baillargeon R, Wu D, Bolz M, Henrich J, Setoh P, Wang J, Laurence S (2013) Early false-belief understanding in traditional non-western societies. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 280:1–6Google Scholar
  7. Bates E, Camaioni L, Volterra V (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Q 21:205–226Google Scholar
  8. Bateson P, Mameli M (2007) The innate and the acquired: useful clusters or a residual distinction from folk biology? Dev Psychobiol 49:818–831CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J (2011) The cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:10918–10925PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brauer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2005) All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. J Comp Psychol 119:145–154CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Brinck I, Liljenfors R (2013) The developmental origin of metacognition. Infant Child Dev 22:85–101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Brown S (1999) The “musilanguage” model of music evolution. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds) The origins of music. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 271–301Google Scholar
  13. Bruner JS (1983) Child’s talk: learning to use language. Norton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Butterworth G (1998) What is special about pointing in babies? In: Simion F, Butterworth G (eds) The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy: from perception to cognition. Psychology Press, Hove, pp 29–40Google Scholar
  15. Butterworth G (2003) Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In: Kita S (ed) Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 9–33Google Scholar
  16. Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn Sci 12:187–192CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Carruthers P (2009) How we know our own minds: the relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behav Brain Sci 32:121–138CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Charles EP, Rivera SM (2009) Object permanence and method of disappearance: looking measures further contradict reaching measures. Dev Sci 12:991–1006CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Tooby J, Cosmides, L (1995) Foreword. In: Baron-Cohen S (ed) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp xi–xviiiGoogle Scholar
  20. Darwin C (1879) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex, 2nd edn. John Murray, LondonGoogle Scholar
  21. Gallese V (2001) The shared manifold hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. J Conscious Stud 8:33–50Google Scholar
  22. Glock H-J (2013) Animal minds: philosophical and scientific aspects. In: Racine TP, Slaney KL (eds) A Wittgensteinian perspective on the role and use of conceptual analysis in psychology. Palgrave Macmillan, Basingstoke, pp 130–152Google Scholar
  23. Gottlieb G (2002) Individual development and evolution: the genesis of novel behaviour. Erlbaum, MahwahGoogle Scholar
  24. Gould SJ, Lewontin RC (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc Lond B 205:581–598CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WD (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Heyes C (2012) New thinking: the evolution of human cognition. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:2091–2096CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hutto DD, Myin E (2013) Radicalizing enactivism: basic minds without content. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  28. Jablonka E, Lamb MJ (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: genetic, epigenetic, behavioural and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  29. Jones SS (2009) The development of imitation in infancy. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:2325–2335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Leavens DA (2012) Pointing: contexts and instrumentality. In: Pika S, Liebal K (eds) Developments in primate gesture research. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 181–197Google Scholar
  31. Leavens DA, Racine TP (2009) Joint attention in apes and humans: are humans unique? J Conscious Stud 16:240–267Google Scholar
  32. Lickliter R, Honeycutt H (2013) A developmental evolutionary framework for psychology. Rev Gen Psychol 17:184–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Luo Y (2011) Do 10-month-old infants understand others’ false beliefs? Cognition 121:289–298CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Mayr E (1961) Cause and effect in biology. Science 134:1501–1506CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Morissette P, Ricard M, Décarie TG (1995) Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: a longitudinal study of comprehension. Br J Dev Psychol 13:163–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Müller U, Racine TP (2010) The development of representation and concepts. In: Overton WF, Lerner RM (eds) Handbook of lifespan human development, vol 1., Methods, biology, neuroscience and cognitive developmentWiley, Hoboken, pp 346–390Google Scholar
  37. Onishi KH, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Sci 308:255–258Google Scholar
  38. Oyama S (2000) The ontogeny of information: developmental systems and evolution, 2nd edn. Duke University Press, DurhamCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Penn DC, Holyoak KJ, Povinelli DJ (2008) Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behav Brain Sci 31:109–130PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Pika S (2008) Evidence for intentional and referential communication in great apes? In: Zlatev J, Racine TP, Sinha C, Itkonen E (eds) The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 165–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Pika S, Bugnyar T (2011) The use of referential gestures in ravens (Corvus corax) in the wild. Nature Commun 2:560CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pinker (1994) The language instinct: the new science of language and mind. Penguin, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. Pinker S (2010) The cognitive niche: coevolution of intelligence, sociality and language. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:8993–8999PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Place UT (2000) The role of the hand in the evolution of language. Psycoloquy. http://www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/psyc
  45. Povinelli DJ, Davis DR (1994) Differences between chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens) in the resting state of the index finger: implications for pointing. J Comp Psychol 108:134–139CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Povinelli DJ, Bering J, Giambrone S (2003) Chimpanzee ‘pointing’: another error of the argument by analogy? In: Kita S (ed) Pointing: where language culture and cognition meet. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 35–68Google Scholar
  47. Racine TP (2012a) Cognitivism, adaptationism, and pointing. In: Pika S, Liebal K (eds) Developments in primate gesture research. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 163–178Google Scholar
  48. Racine TP (2012b) Getting beyond rich and lean views of joint attention. In: Seemann A (ed) Joint attention: new developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 21–42Google Scholar
  49. Racine TP (2013) How useful are the concepts ‘innate’ and ‘adaptation’ for explaining human development? Hum Dev 56:141–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Racine TP, Carpendale JIM (2007) The role of shared practice in joint attention. Br J Dev Psychol 25:3–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Racine TP, Wereha TJ, Leavens DA (2012) To what extent nonhuman primates are intersubjective and why. In: Foolen A, Lüdtke U, Racine TP, Zlatev J (eds) Moving ourselves, moving others: motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 221–242Google Scholar
  52. Slaney KL, Racine TP (2011) On the ambiguity of concept use in psychology: Is the concept ‘concept’ a useful concept? J Theor Philos Psychol 31:73–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tallis RF (2011) Aping mankind: neuromania, Darwinitis, and the misrepresentation of humanity. Acumen, DurhamGoogle Scholar
  54. Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschirift für Tierpsychologie 20:410–413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomasello M (1995) Joint attention as social cognition. In: Moore C, Dunham P (eds) Joint attention: its origins and role in development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 103–130Google Scholar
  56. Tomasello M (2006) Why don’t apes point? In: Enfield NJ, Levinson SC (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and interaction. Berg, Oxford, pp 506–524Google Scholar
  57. Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  58. Tomasello M (2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  59. Tomasello M, Carpenter M (2007) Shared intentionality. Dev Sci 10:121–125CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28:675–735PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Liszkowski U (2007) A new look at infant pointing. Child Dev 78:705–722CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Tooby J, Cosmides L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J (eds) The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 19–136Google Scholar
  63. Trevarthen C (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 321–347Google Scholar
  64. Trevarthen C (2011) What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being. Infant Child Dev 20:119–135Google Scholar
  65. Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture, and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic Press, London, pp 183–229Google Scholar
  66. Wereha TJ, Racine TP (2012) Evolution, development, and human social cognition. Rev Philos Psychol 3:559–579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Whiten A, Erdal D (2012) The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philos Trans R Soc B 367:2119–2129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wilkins D (2003) Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms). In: Kita S (ed) Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 171–216 Google Scholar
  69. Wynn K (1998) An evolved capacity for number. In: Cummins D, Allen C (eds) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 107–126Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Timothy P. Racine
    • 1
    Email author
  • Tyler J. Wereha
    • 1
  • Olga Vasileva
    • 1
  • Donna Tafreshi
    • 1
  • Joseph J. Thompson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologySimon Fraser UniversityBurnabyCanada

Personalised recommendations