Advertisement

Reevaluating Chimpanzee Vocal Signals: Toward a Multimodal Account of the Origins of Human Communication

  • Adam SeeEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Interdisciplinary Evolution Research book series (IDER, volume 1)

Abstract

The vocalizations of chimpanzees have long been thought to be largely genetically predetermined and therefore unlearnable, involuntarily produced, and broadcast indiscriminately. Tomasello (2008) has recently written that, while chimpanzee vocalizations share these constraints and limitations with the vocal displays of all other non-human animals, the attention-getting gestures of chimpanzees are an “evolutionary novelty” because they are, in his estimation, capable of being produced intentionally. As such, chimpanzee gestures are highly significant to discussions of animal cognition and the evolution of human communication. This chapter challenges Tomasello’s grounds for restricting this evolutionary novelty to the gestural modality. I argue that, in fact, recent evidence suggests that there is a significant functional difference between certain chimpanzee vocalizations and the vocal displays of other animals and that, based on Tomasello’s own criteria for intentionality, gestures do not appear to have a monopoly on intentional communication in chimpanzees. Ultimately, this chapter aims to provide grounds for a multimodal account of the evolution of human communication. I conclude by suggesting that although there is reason to doubt that chimpanzees can communicate intentionally, there is no more reason to doubt this ability in the vocal modality than there is in the gestural modality.

Keywords

Tomasello Vocalizations Intentionality Chimpanzee Animal communication Evolution of language 

References

  1. Arbib MA, Liebal K, Pika S (2008) Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Curr Anthropol 49:1053–1076PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bates E, Camaioni L, Volterra V (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill Palmer Q 21:205–226Google Scholar
  3. Berlin P (2006) Voice processing in human and non-human primates. Philos Trans R Soc B 361:2091–2107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bodamer MD, Gardner RA (2002) How cross-fostered chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) initiate and maintain conversations. J Comp Psychol 116:12–26PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2005) Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. Linguist Rev 22:135–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2007) Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2010) Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang 115:92–100PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Clark AP, Wrangham RW (1994) Chimpanzee arrival pant-hoots: do they signify food or status? Int J Primatol 15:185–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Crockford C, Boesch C (2003) Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Anim Behav 66:115–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Crockford C, Boesch C (2005) Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 142:397–421Google Scholar
  12. Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigilant L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Mundry R, Zuberbühler K (2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Curr Biol 22:142–146PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dennett DC (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  15. Fehér O, Wang H, Saar S, Mitra PP, Tchernichovshi O (2009) De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature 459:564–568PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Flombaum JI, Santos LR (2005) Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Curr Biol 15:447–452PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Galef BG (1976) Social transmission of acquired behavior: a discussion of tradition and social learning in vertebrates. In: Rosenblatt JS, Hinde RA, Shaw E, Beer C (eds) Advances in the study of behavior, vol 6. Academic Press, New York, pp 77–100Google Scholar
  18. Golinkoff RM (1993) When is communication a ‘meeting of minds’? J Child Lang 20:199–207PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  20. Halloran AR (2012) The song of the ape: understanding the language of chimpanzees. St. Martin’s Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  21. Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J (2008) Constraints in primate vocal production. In: Griebel U, Oller K (eds) The evolution of communicative creativity: from fixed signals to contextual flexibility. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 93–119Google Scholar
  22. Hopkins WD, Taglialatela JP, Leavens DA (2007) Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalisations to capture the attention of a human. Anim Behav 73:281–286PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hostetter AB, Cantero M, Hopkins WD (2001) Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 115:337–343PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hostetter AB, Russell JL, Freeman H, Hopkins WD (2007) Now you see me, now you don’t: evidence that chimpanzees understand the role of the eyes in attention. Anim Cogn 10:55–62PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Janik VW, Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals. Adv Study Behav 26:59–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kojima S, Izumi A, Ceugniet M (2003) Identification of vocalizers by pant hoots, pant grunts and screams in a chimpanzee. Primates 44:225–230PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Krause MA, Fouts RS (1997) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) pointing: hand shapes, accuracy, and the role of eye gaze. J Comp Psychol 111:330–336PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Laporte MNC, Zuberbuhler K (2010) Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Anim Behav 80:467–473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Leavens D, Hopkins WD (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Dev Psychol 34(5):813–822PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Leavens DA, Hostetter AB, Wesley MJ, Hopkins WD (2004) Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim Behav 67:467–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2005) Intentionality as measured in the persistence and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Child Dev 76:291–306PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2010) Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 13:33–40PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Levréro F, Mathevon N (2013) Vocal signature in wild infant chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 75:324–332PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Liebal K, Pika S, Call J, Tomasello M (2004) To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interact Stud 5:199–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lurz R (2011) Mindreading animals: the debate over what animals know about other minds. MIT Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Marler P (1991) Song learning behavior: the interface with neuroethology. Trends Neurosci 5:199–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Marshall AJ, Wrangham RW, Arcadi AC (1999) Does learning affect the structure of vocalisations in chimpanzees? Anim Behav 58:825–830PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mitani JC, Hunley KL, Murdoch ME (1999) Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: a reassessment. Am J Primatol 47:133–151PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mitani JC, Nishida T (1993) Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 45:735–746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pollick AS, de Waal FBM (2007) Ape gestures and language evolution. PNAS. http://www.pnas.org/content/104/19/8184.long. Accessed 24 Oct 2013
  41. Povinelli DJ, Eddy TJ (1996) What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 61:56–97Google Scholar
  42. Povinelli DJ, Vonk J (2006) We don’t need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee’s mind. In: Hurley S (ed) Rational animals. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  43. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Russell JL, Braccini S, Buehler N, Kachin MJ, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD (2005) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) intentional communication is not contingent upon food. Anim Cogn 8:263–272PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schel AM, Townsend SW, Machanda Z, Zuberbuhler K, Slocombe KE (2013a) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE 8(10):e76674PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE (2013b) Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Anim Behav 86(5):955–965Google Scholar
  47. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2003) Signalers and receivers in animal communication. Annu Rev Psychol 54:145–173PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Slocombe KE, Kaller T, Turman L, Townsend SW, Papworth S et al (2010) Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1959–1966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K (2007) Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104:17228–17233PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Taglialatela JP, Reamer L, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD (2012) Social learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees. Biol Lett. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22438489. Accessed 24 Oct 2013
  51. Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD (2011) Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PLoS ONE 6(4):e18852PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Theall LA, Povinelli DJ (1999) Do chimpanzees tailor their gestural signals to fit the attentional state of others? Anim Cogn 2:207–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Tomasello M (1996) Do apes ape? In: Heyes CM, Galef BG (eds) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 319–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomasello M, Zuberbuhler K (2002) Primate vocal and gestural communication. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  56. Townsend SW, Deschner T, Zuberbühler K (2008) Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS ONE 3(6):e2431PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Wich SA, de Vries H (2006) Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proc R Soc B 273:735–740Google Scholar
  58. Wilson ML, Hauser MD, Wrangham RW (2001) Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Anim Behav 61:1203–1216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Winter P, Handley P, Ploog D, Schott D (1973) Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal conditions and under acoustic isolation. Anim Behav 47:230–239Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyGraduate Center of the City University of New YorkNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations