Skip to main content

Clinical Pragmatics and Theory of Mind

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Perspectives on Linguistic Pragmatics

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology ((PEPRPHPS,volume 2))

Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM) describes the cognitive ability to attribute mental states both to one’s own mind and to the minds of others. In recent years, ToM has been credited with playing a significant role in developmental and acquired pragmatic disorders. In this way, ToM deficits have been linked to pragmatic deficits in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (e.g. Martin and McDonald, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328, 2004), emotional and behavioural disorders (e.g. Buitelaar et al., Development and Psychopathology, 11, 39–58, 1999), intellectual disability (e.g. Cornish et al., Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 372–378, 2005), right-hemisphere damage (e.g. Winner et al., Brain and Language, 62, 89–106, 1998), schizophrenia (e.g. Brüne and Bodenstein, Schizophrenia Research, 75, 233–239, 2005), traumatic brain injury (e.g. McDonald and Flanagan, Neuropsychology, 18, 572–579, 2004) and neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease (e.g. Cuerva et al., Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 14, 153–158, 2001). In this chapter, I examine the central role of ToM reasoning in utterance interpretation. The chapter addresses what is known about ToM development during childhood and adolescence as well as changes in ToM skills as part of the aging process. The role of ToM in developmental and acquired pragmatic disorders is discussed. The contribution of ToM research into pragmatic disorders is critically evaluated. Finally, several ToM theories are examined. The question is addressed of which, if any, of these theories is able to capture the pragmatic features of utterance interpretation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In a study of the conversational responsiveness of children with SLI, Bishop et al. (2000: 177) make this same point as follows: ‘this study lends support to the notion that there is a subset of the language-impaired population who have broader communicative impairments, extending beyond basic difficulties in mastering language form, reflecting difficulty in responding to and expressing communicative intents’.

  2. 2.

    Abbeduto et al.’s finding lends support to the second of the views delineated by Tager-Flusberg (2000) in the main text. This is the view that language and verbal interaction play a vital role in the development of ToM.

  3. 3.

    Some studies have failed to find evidence of ToM impairments in patients with TBI. Bach et al. (2006) reported that the patients with TBI with and without behavioural disturbance in their study were unimpaired on ToM tasks.

  4. 4.

    There is some basis for this claim in the findings of several studies that different symptom profiles in schizophrenia appear to be associated with certain patterns of ToM and pragmatic deficits (e.g. Corcoran and Frith 1996; Montag et al. 2011).

  5. 5.

    Sally-Anne experiments derive their name from the two dolls—Sally and Anne—that are used in these false-belief tests. See an account of these tests by Baron-Cohen et al. (1985) in Sect. 3.

  6. 6.

    There is some evidence that these different ToM tests may not examine the same mentalising skills. For example, Spek et al. (2010) found low or absent correlations between the Eyes test and three other ToM tests used in their study of adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome.

  7. 7.

    It is worth remarking that the original proponent of modularity—Fodor (1983)—would disavow the attempt to locate ToM skills within a domain-specific cognitive module. For Fodor, such skills are located within a non-modularised central system in the mind. Frye (2000: 149) makes this same point as follows: ‘An odd aspect of the view that theory of mind is domain specific is that it is one Fodor’s (1983) own approach to modularity would explicitly disclaim’.

  8. 8.

    That one theory theorist, Alison Gopnik, cannot even conceive of modularity as permitting of developmental change is evident in her use of the word ‘indefeasible’ when characterising a ToM module. A ToM module, she states, is ‘a genetically-determined and indefeasible way of understanding the mind’ (Gopnik et al. 2000: 51).

References

  • Abbeduto, L., Pavetto, M., Kesin, E., Weissman, M. D., Karadottir, S., O’Brien, A., et al. (2001). The linguistic and cognitive profile of Down syndrome: Evidence from a comparison with fragile X syndrome. Down’s Syndrome Research and Practice, 7, 9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abbeduto, L., Short-Meyerson, K., Benson, G., & Dolish, J. (2004). Relationship between theory of mind and language ability in children and adolescents with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 48, 150–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M. M., Richmond, E. K., Amman, A., Beth, P., Weissman, M. D., et al. (2006). Collaboration in referential communication: Comparison of youth with Down syndrome or fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 111, 170–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abbeduto, L., Murphy, M. M., Kover, S. T., Giles, N. D., Karadottir, S., Amman, A., et al. (2008). Signaling noncomprehension of language: A comparison of fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 113, 214–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adachi, T., Koeda, T., Hirabayashi, S., Maeoka, Y., Shiota, M., Wright, E. C., et al. (2004). The metaphor and sarcasm scenario test: A new instrument to help differentiate high functioning pervasive developmental disorder from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain & Development, 26, 301–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, C., Clarke, E., & Haynes, R. (2009). Inference and sentence comprehension in children with specific or pragmatic language impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44, 301–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adrián, J. E., Clemente, R. A., & Villanueva, L. (2007). Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78, 1052–1067.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annaz, D., Van Herwegen, J., Thomas, M., Fishman, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Rundblad, G. (2009). Comprehension of metaphor and metonymy in children with Williams syndrome. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44, 962–978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, K., Kose, S., Williams, L., Woolard, A., & Heckers, S. (2012). Impaired associative inference in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 622–629

    Google Scholar 

  • Averbeck, B. B., Evans, S., Chouhan, V., Bristow, E., & Shergill, S. S. (2011). Probabilistic learning and inference in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 127, 115–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bach, L. J., Happé, F., Fleminger, S., & David, A. S. (2006). Intact theory of mind in TBI with behavioural disturbance. Brain and Cognition, 60, 196–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 3–20). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (2000). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beeke, S. (2003). ‘I suppose’ as a resource for the construction of turns at talk in agrammatic aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17, 291–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthiaume, K. S., Lorch, E. P., & Milich, R. (2010). Getting clued in: Inferential processing and comprehension monitoring in boys with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 14, 31–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bignell, S., & Cain, K. (2007). Pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension in groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 499–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the children’s communication checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children’s communication checklist in a clinical setting. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 809–818.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Chan, J., Adams, C., Hartley, J., & Weir, F. (2000). Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: Evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 177–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blake, M. L. (2006). Clinical relevance of discourse characteristics after right hemisphere brain damage. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 255–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodden, M. E., Mollenhauer, B., Trenkwalder, C., Cabanel, N., Eggert, K. M., Unger, M. M., et al. (2010). Affective and cognitive theory of mind in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 16, 466–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borod, J. C., Rorie, K. D., Pick, L. H., Bloom, R. L., Andelman, F., Campbell, A. L., et al. (2000). Verbal pragmatics following unilateral stroke: Emotional content and valence. Neuropsychology, 14, 112–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosco, F. M., Friedman, O., & Leslie, A. M. (2006). Recognition of pretend and real actions in play by 1- and 2-year-olds: Early success and why they fail. Cognitive Development, 21, 3–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., & Adams, C. (2005). Semantic and inferencing abilities in children with communication disorders. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40, 49–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H., & Stringfellow, A. (1999). Making requests: Illustrations of how right-hemisphere brain damage can affect discourse production. Brain and Language, 68, 442–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brundage, S. (1996). Comparison of proverb interpretations provided by right-hemisphere-damaged adults and adults with probable dementia of the Alzheimer type. Clinical Aphasiology, 24, 215–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brüne, M., & Bodenstein, L. (2005). Proverb comprehension reconsidered—‘theory of mind’ and the pragmatic use of language in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 75, 233–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buitelaar, J. K., van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., & van der Gaag, R. J. (1999). Theory of mind and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 39–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlomagno, S., Santoro, A., Menditti, A., Pandolfi, M., & Marini, A. (2005). Referential communication in Alzheimer’s type dementia. Cortex, 41, 520–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P., & Smith, P. K. (1996). Introduction. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 1–8). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, I., Pini, A., Alberoni, M., Liverta-Sempio, O., Baglio, F., Massaro, D., et al. (2011). Mapping levels of theory of mind in Alzheimer’s disease: A preliminary study. Aging & Mental Health, 15, 157–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, S. B., Ulatowska, H. K., Franklin, L. R., Shobe, A. E., Thompson, J. L., & McIntire, D. D. (1997). Proverb interpretation in fluent aphasia and Alzheimer’s disease: Implications beyond abstract thinking. Aphasiology, 11, 337–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheang, H., & Pell, M. (2006). A study of humour and communicative intention following right hemisphere stroke. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 20, 447–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coelho, C. A., & Flewellyn, L. (2003). Longitudinal assessment of coherence in an adult with fluent aphasia. Aphasiology, 17, 173–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coelho, C. A., Youse, K., & Le, K. (2002). Conversational discourse in closed-head-injured and non-brain-injured adults. Aphasiology, 16, 659–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & van der Lely, H. K. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 28–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, R. (2003). Inductive reasoning and the understanding of intention in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8, 223–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, R., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Conversational conduct and the symptoms of schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 305–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornish, K., Burack, J. A., Rahman, A., Munir, F., Russo, N., & Grant, C. (2005). Theory of mind deficits in children with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49, 372–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuerva, A. G., Sake, L., Kuzis, G., Tiberti, C., Dorrego, F., & Starkstein, S. E. (2001). Theory of mind and pragmatic abilities in dementia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 14, 153–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2005). Pragmatics: A multidisciplinary perspective. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2007a). Pragmatics and adult language disorders: Past achievements and future directions. Seminars in Speech and Language, 28, 98–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2007b). Clinical pragmatics: A field in search of phenomena? Language & Communication, 27, 396–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2008). Clinical linguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2009). Clinical pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2010). Clinical pragmatics. In L. Cummings (Ed.), The Routledge pragmatics encyclopedia (pp. 40–43). Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2011). Pragmatic disorders and their social impact. Pragmatics and Society, 2, 17–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2012a). Pragmatic disorders. In H. -J. Schmid (Ed.), Cognitive pragmatics. [Handbook of pragmatics, vol.4 ], Berlin and Boston: Walter de Gruyter, 291–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2012b). Theorising context: The case of clinical pragmatics. In R. Finkbeiner, J. Meibauer & P. Schumacher (Eds.), What is context? Theoretical and experimental evidence, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 55–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2014a). Clinical pragmatics. In Y. Huang (Ed.), Oxford handbook of pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2014b). Pragmatic disorders and theory of mind. In L. Cummings (Ed.), Cambridge Handbook of communication disorders (pp. 559–577). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, L. (2014c). Communication disorders. Houndmills, Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutica, I., Bucciarelli, M., & Bara, B. (2006). Neuropragmatics: Extralinguistic pragmatic ability is better preserved in left-hemisphere-damaged patients than in right-hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 98, 12–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • David, N., Aumann, C., Bewernick, B. H., Santos, N. S., Lehnhardt, F. G., & Vogeley, K. (2010). Investigation of mentalizing and visuospatial perspective taking for self and other in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 290–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I.-M. (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Research, 3, 311–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M., & Barnes, M. A. (2001). Comparison of literal, inferential, and intentional text comprehension in children with mild or severe closed head injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 456–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donno, R., Parker, G., Gilmour, J., & Skuse, D. H. (2010). Social communication deficits in disruptive primary-school children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 282–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, J. M. (2010). Relation of executive functioning to pragmatic outcome following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 365–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrant, B. M., Fletcher, J., & Maybery, M. T. (2006). Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: Evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. Child Development, 77, 1842–1853.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Black, S. E. (2009). False-belief understanding in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 489–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferstl, E. C., Guthke, T., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). Text comprehension after brain injury: Left prefrontal lesions affect inference processes. Neuropsychology, 16, 292–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyereisen, P., Berrewaerts, J., & Hupet, M. (2007). Pragmatic skills in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis by means of a referential communication task. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frye, D. (2000). Theory of mind, domain specificity, and reasoning. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 149–167). East Sussex: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H. M., & Embrechts, M. (2008). Language profiles in ASD, SLI, and ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1931–1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, Z. C., Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., Happé, F., Richardson, A., & Neary, D. (2007). Inferring thought and action in motor neurone disease. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1196–1207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilmour, J., Hill, B., Place, M., & Skuse, D. H. (2004). Social communication deficits in conduct disorder: A clinical and community survey. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 967–978.

    Google Scholar 

  • Girardi, A., Macpherson, S. E., & Abrahams, S. (2011). Deficits in emotional and social cognition in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neuropsychology, 25, 53–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 169–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Golan, Y. (2008). The ‘Reading the Mind in Films’ task [child version]: Complex emotion and mental state recognition in children with and without autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1534–1541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold, R., Faust, M., & Goldstein, A. (2010). Semantic integration during metaphor comprehension in Asperger syndrome. Brain and Language, 113, 124–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. (1993). The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 15–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., Capps, L., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2000). Early theories of mind: What the theory theory can tell us about autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 50–72). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. M. (1986). Folk psychology as simulation. Mind & Language, 1, 158–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. M. (1996). ‘Radical’ simulationism. In P. Carruthers & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 11–21). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant, C. M., Apperly, I., & Oliver, C. (2007). Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with fragile X syndrome? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 17–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L., Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain, 125, 752–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, R., Friedman, O., Ween, J., Winner, E., Happe, F., & Brownell, H. (2006). Theory of mind and the right cerebral hemisphere: Refining the scope of impairment. Laterality, 11, 195–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, C. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism, 9, 157–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. F., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2009). Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Cognition, 113, 37–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (1999). Acquired ‘theory of mind’ impairments following stroke. Cognition, 70, 211–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., & Summers, F. (2006). Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1623–1628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T., & McNamara, P. (2010). Pragmatic comprehension deficit in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, 388–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2005). Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development, 76, 356–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. M., Franco, F., Dolins, F., & Sewell, A. (1995). Toddlers’ reactions to negative emotion displays: Forming models of relationships. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 273–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, A. E., Rowe, M. L., & Mervis, C. B. (2009). Referential communication skills of children with Williams syndrome: Understanding when messages are not adequate. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114, 85–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. R., Miller, J., & Tallal, P. (2001). Use of cognitive state predicates by language-impaired children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 349–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. D., & Schwartz, I. S. (2009). When asking questions is not enough: An observational study of social communication differences in high functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 432–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasher, A., Batori, G., Soroker, N., Graves, D., & Zaidel, E. (1999). Effects of right- and left-hemisphere damage on understanding conversational implicatures. Brain and Language, 68, 566–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A., Jesso, S., Harciarek, M., Blair, M., & McMonagle, P. (2010). What is semantic dementia?: A cohort study of diagnostic features and clinical boundaries. Archives of Neurology, 67, 483–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinman, J., Marciano, P. L., & Ault, R. L. (2001). Advanced theory of mind in high-functioning adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 29–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperberg, G. R., McGuire, P. K., & David, A. S. (2000). Sensitivity to linguistic anomalies in spoken sentences: A case study approach to understanding thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 30, 345–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P. B., & Catts, S. V. (2002). Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: The role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading. Psychological Medicine, 32, 1273–1284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39, 45–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddle, B., & Nettle, D. (2006). Higher-order theory of mind and social competence in school-age children. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 231–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2010). Impaired performance on see-know tasks amongst children with autism: Evidence of specific difficulties with theory of mind or domain-general task factors? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 479–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 239–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lough, S., Kipps, C. M., Treise, C., Watson, P., Blair, J. R., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). Social reasoning, emotion and empathy in frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia, 44, 950–958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loukusa, S., Leinonen, E., Kuusikko, S., Jussila, K., Mattila, M.-L., Ryder, N., et al. (2007). Use of context in pragmatic language comprehension by children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1049–1059.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLennan, D. L., Cornis-Pop, M., Picon-Nieto, L., & Sigford, B. (2002). The prevalence of pragmatic communication impairments in traumatic brain injury. Premier Outlook, 3, 38–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marini, A., Carlomagno, S., Caltagirone, C., & Nocentini, U. (2005). The role played by the right hemisphere in the organization of complex textual structures. Brain and Language, 93, 46–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marini, A., Spoletini, I., Rubino, I. A., Ciuffa, M., Bria, P., Martinotti, G., et al. (2008). The language of schizophrenia: An analysis of micro and macrolinguistic abilities and their neuropsychological correlates. Schizophrenia Research, 105, 144–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., Moore, C., & Tannock, R. (2009). Empathy and social perspective taking in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A., Moulson, J. M., Muncer, A.-M., & Taylor, L. A. (2002). Does performance on theory of mind tasks decline in old age? British Journal of Psychology, 93, 465–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, M., Di Michele, V., Pollice, R., Casacchia, M., & Roncone, R. (2008). Pragmatic language and theory of mind deficits in people with schizophrenia and their relatives. Psychopathology, 41, 254–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, S., & Flanagan, S. (2004). Social perception deficits after traumatic brain injury: Interaction between emotion recognition, mentalizing ability, and social communication. Neuropsychology, 18, 572–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • McInnes, A., Humphries, T., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2003). Listening comprehension and working memory are impaired in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder irrespective of language impairment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 427–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, P., & Durso, R. (2003). Pragmatic communication skills in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 84, 414–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milders, M., Ietswaart, M., & Crawford, J. R. (2006). Impairments in theory of mind shortly after traumatic brain injury and at 1-year follow-up. Neuropsychology, 20, 400–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. A. (2001). False belief understanding in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 73–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. A. (2004). False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39, 191–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mo, S., Su, Y., Chan, R. C. K., & Liu, J. (2008). Comprehension of metaphor and irony in schizophrenia during remission: The role of theory of mind and IQ. Psychiatry Research, 157, 21–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monetta, L., & Pell, M. D. (2007). Effects of verbal working memory deficits on metaphor comprehension in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 101, 80–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monetta, L., Grindrod, C. M., & Pell, M. D. (2009). Irony comprehension and theory of mind deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Cortex, 45, 972–981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montag, C., Dziobek, I., Richter, I. S., Neuhaus, K., Lehmann, A., Sylla, R., et al. (2011). Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: Evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Research, 186, 203–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller, F., Simion, A., Reviriego, E., Galera, C., Mazaux, J.-M., Barat, M., et al. (2010). Exploring theory of mind after severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 46, 1088–1099.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, M. M., & Abbeduto, L. (2007). Gender differences in repetitive language in fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51, 387–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mutter, B., Alcorn, M. B., & Welsh, M. (2006). Theory of mind and executive function: Working-memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102, 819–835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, P. S. (1979). Profiles of communication deficits in patients with right cerebral hemisphere damage: Implications for diagnosis and treatment. In Clinical aphasiology conference (pp. 38–46). Phoenix, AZ: BRK Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, P. B., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2008). Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of theory of mind. Developmental Science, 11, 847–852.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). 15-month-old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. Acta Psychologica, 124, 106–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C. (2001). Comprehension of metaphors and idioms in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study. Brain, 124, 1450–1460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (2001). Understanding metaphors and idioms: A single-case neuropsychological study in a person with Down syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 516–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papagno, C., Curti, R., Rizzo, S., Crippa, F., & Colombo, M. R. (2006). Is the right hemisphere involved in idiom comprehension? A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 20, 598–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pardini, M., & Nichelli, P. F. (2009). Age-related decline in mentalizing skills across adult life span. Experimental Aging Research, 35, 98–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelc, K., Kornreich, C., Foisy, M.-L., & Dan, B. (2006). Recognition of emotional facial expressions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatric Neurology, 35, 93–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rinaldi, W. (2000). Pragmatic comprehension in secondary school-aged students with specific developmental language disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ripich, D. N., Carpenter, B. D., & Ziol, E. W. (2000). Conversational cohesion patterns in men and women with Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35, 49–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roncone, R., Mazza, M., Frangou, I., De Risio, A., Ussorio, D., Tozzini, C., et al. (2004). Rehabilitation of theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: A pilot study of metacognitive strategies in group treatment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation: An International Journal, 14, 421–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseaux, M., Sève, A., Vallet, M., Pasquier, F., & Mackowiak-Cordoliani, M. A. (2010). An analysis of communication in conversation in patients with dementia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3884–3890.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., & Clements, W. A. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mother’s mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 189–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, N., Leinonen, E., & Schulz, J. (2008). Cognitive approach to assessing pragmatic language comprehension in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 427–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saldert, C., Fors, A., Ströberg, S., & Hartelius, L. (2010). Comprehension of complex discourse in different stages of Huntington’s disease. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 656–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saltzman, J., Strauss, E., Hunter, M., & Archibald, S. (2000). Theory of mind and executive functions in normal human aging and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 781–788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Modularity, development and ‘theory of mind’. Mind & Language, 14, 131–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2006). Aphasia, language, and theory of mind. Social Neuroscience, 1, 167–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spanoudis, G., Natsopoulos, D., & Panayiotou, G. (2007). Mental verbs and pragmatic language difficulties. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42, 487–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speechley, W. J., Murray, C. B., McKay, R. M., Munz, M. T., & Ngan, E. T. C. (2010). A failure of conflict to modulate dual-stream processing may underlie the formation and maintenance of delusions. European Psychiatry, 25, 80–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spek, A. A., Scholte, E. M., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2010). Theory of mind in adults with HFA and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 280–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind & Language, 17, 3–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sudhalter, V., & Belser, R. C. (2001). Conversational characteristics of children with fragile X syndrome: Tangential language. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 389–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, S., & Ruffman, T. (2004). Social understanding: How does it fare with advancing years? British Journal of Psychology, 95, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: Intact or impaired? American Journal of Mental Retardation, 104, 523–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Can adolescents with Williams syndrome tell the difference between lies and jokes? Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 85–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surian, L., Baron-Cohen, S., & Van der Lely, H. (1996). Are children with autism deaf to Gricean maxims? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1, 55–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Surian, L., & Siegal, M. (2001). Sources of performance on theory of mind tasks in right hemisphere-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 78, 224–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J. (2000). Teaching theory of mind to individuals with autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 442–456). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D., McLaughlin, E., Moore, C., & Morine, S. (1997). Integrating relationship constructs and emotional experience into false belief tasks in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 423–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 124–149). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tavano, A., Sponda, S., Fabbro, F., Perlini, C., Rambaldelli, G., Ferro, A., et al. (2008). Specific linguistic and pragmatic deficits in Italian patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 102, 53–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (1997). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 68, 436–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tényi, T., Herold, R., Szili, I. M., & Trixler, M. (2002). Schizophrenics show a failure in the decoding of violations of conversational implicatures. Psychopathology, 35, 25–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titone, D., Ditman, T., Holzman, P. S., Eichenbaum, H., & Levy, D. L. (2004). Transitive inference in schizophrenia: Impairments in relational memory organization. Schizophrenia Research, 68, 235–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Togher, L., & Hand, L. (1998). Use of politeness markers with different communication partners: An investigation of five subjects with traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 12, 755–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Bekinschtein, T., & Manes, F. (2009). A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 132, 1299–1309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkstra, L. S., Dixon, T. M., & Baker, K. K. (2004). Theory of mind and social beliefs in adolescents with traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 19, 245–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R., & Siegal, M. (2000). Evidence for cognition without grammar from causal reasoning and ‘theory of mind’ in an agrammatic aphasic patient. Current Biology, 10, 723–726.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R., Siegal, M., & Want, S. C. (2001). Severe impairment in grammar does not preclude theory of mind. Neurocase, 7, 489–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, A. C., Painter, K. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2001). Longitudinal relations between 2-year-olds’ language and 4-year-olds’ theory of mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 449–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weed, E. (2008). Theory of mind impairment in right hemisphere damage: A review of the evidence. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 414–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weed, E., McGregor, W., Nielsen, J. F., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, U. (2010). Theory of mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What’s the story? Brain and Language, 113, 65–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Lagattuta, K. H. (2000). Developing understandings of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 21–49). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D., & Happé, F. (2010). Representing intentions in self and other: Studies of autism and typical development. Developmental Science, 13, 307–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, E., Brownell, H., Happé, F., Blum, A., & Pincus, D. (1998). Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 62, 89–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Solomonica-Levi, D., Shulman, C., & Pilowsky, T. (1996). Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 1003–1014.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Louise Cummings .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cummings, L. (2013). Clinical Pragmatics and Theory of Mind. In: Capone, A., Lo Piparo, F., Carapezza, M. (eds) Perspectives on Linguistic Pragmatics. Perspectives in Pragmatics, Philosophy & Psychology, vol 2. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01014-4_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics