Skip to main content

The Spell of Kuhn on Psychology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Clinical Psychology and the Philosophy of Science
  • 3777 Accesses

Abstract

In their meta-scientific studies of psychology, psychologists often use what they take to be the views of the historian of science, Thomas Kuhn. Although a critical examination of psychology or aspects of psychology is laudatory, psychologists also need to accurately understand and to assume a critical stance toward the meta-scientific views that they employ. In this chapter, the views of the historian of science, Thomas Kuhn, are described and examined. The following major questions are addressed: What were Kuhn’s investigative methods? What are his views of science? What exactly do Kuhn’s conclusions about science mean? How does Kuhn rely on psychology? and, What does Kuhn have to say about psychology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgment. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and men. Carnegie: Pittsburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. A., & Byrne, D. (1987). Social psychology: understanding human interaction. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1949). On the perception of incongruity: a paradigm. Journal of Personality, 18, 206–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Postman, L., & Rodriguez, J. (1951). Expectations and the perception of color. American Journal of Psychology, 64, 216–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, I. S. (1972). Psychology and Kuhn’s concept of paradigm. Journal of Behavioral Science, 1, 193–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. R. (1978). The structure of psychological revolutions. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 14, 57–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, S. R., & Salarmon, R. (1988). Kuhn’s Structure of Scientific Revolutions in the psychological journal literature, 1969–1983: a descriptive study. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 9, 415–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. K. (1970). Consolations for the specialist. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against method. New York: New Left Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gholson, B., & Barker, P. (1985). Kuhn Lakatos, and Laudan: applications in the history of physics and psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. (1988). Explaining science: A cognitive approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: a cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastrof, A. H. (1950). The influence of suggestion on the relationship between stimulus size and perceived distance. Journal of Psychology, 29, 195–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houts, A. C. (1988, August). What’s wrong with psychologism? Toward a behavior analytic psychology of science. Paper presented at the 96th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New Jersey: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirton, M. (1976). Adapters and innovators: a description and measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970a). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970b). Logic of discovery or psychology of research? In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970c). Reflections on my critics. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1974). Second thoughts on paradigms. In F. Suppe (Ed.), The structure of scientific theories (pp. 459–482). Urbana, Illinois: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1976). Foreword. In L. Fleck (Ed.), Genesis and development of a scientific fact. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, L. (1977). Progress and its problems. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masterman, M. (1970). The nature of a paradigm. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFall, R. M. (1991). Manifesto for a science of clinical psychology. Clinical Psychologist, 44, 75–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, 313–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulkay, M. & Gilbert, G. N. (1981). Putting philosophy to work, philosophy of the social sciences (Vol. 11, pp. 389–408).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Donohue, W. (1990). Review of paradigms in behavior therapy: present and promise. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 11, 105–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palermo, D. S. (1971). Is a scientific revolution taking place in psychology? Science Studies, 1, 135–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, G. L. (1981). Historical self-understanding in the social sciences: the use of Thomas Kuhn in psychology. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 11, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The “false consensus effect”: an egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rychlak, J. F., Carlsen, N. L., & Dunning, L. P. (1974). Personal adjustment and the free recall of materials with affectively positive or negative meaningfulness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 480–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M. (1935). A study of some factors in perception. Archives of Psychology, 27, 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stratton, G. M. (1897). Vision without inversion of the retinal image. Psychological Review, 6, 341–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. In: D. Kahneman, P. Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.) Judgment under uncertainty (pp. 3–20). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, N. (1971). Is a scientific revolution taking place in psychology? Doubts and reservations. Science Studies, 1, 407–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, J. W. N. (1970). Against ‘normal science’. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weimer, W. B., & Palermo, D. S. (1973). Paradigms and normal science in psychology. Science Studies, 3, 211–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, L. P. (1970). Normal science, scientific revolutions and the history of science. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to William O’Donohue .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

O’Donohue, W. (2013). The Spell of Kuhn on Psychology. In: Clinical Psychology and the Philosophy of Science. Springer, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-00185-2_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics