Advertisement

From Emotions to Artifacts: Four Modes of Fulfilling Life-Relevant Tasks

Chapter
  • 86 Downloads
Part of the Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics book series (SAPERE, volume 56)

Abstract

Following the taxonomy of four modes of cognition sketched by Roberto Casati (a syncretism of dual system theories of reasoning and distributed cognition), I describe four modes of addressing evolutionary salient tasks, i.e. those tasks like foraging and mating on which the survival of the species hinges upon. According to an influential tradition, (1) emotions are toolkits designed by evolution to efficiently address biologically relevant challenges. However, Homo sapiens address some of these challenges by (2) rearranging the workings of emotions by means of higher-cognitive faculties. Moreover, still other challenges are dealt with by (3) scaffolding or even totally (4) offloading the relevant tasks to external resources, such as artifacts.

Notes

Acknowledgements

I am indebted to Colin Klein, Irene Papa, Giulia Piredda, Agostino Pinna Pintor and Sarah Songhorian for their comments on previous versions of this paper. This version also benefitted from Marco Fasoli and Valetina Petrolini’s insightful comments. Last but not least, I sincerely thank Tommaso W. Bertolotti for organizing the workshop and putting up this volume.

References

  1. Adolphs R, Anderson DJ (2018) The neuroscience of emotion: a new synthesis. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett LF (2017) The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc Cogn Affect neurosci 12(1):1–23Google Scholar
  3. Buss DM (2013) Sex Jealousy Psychol Topi 22(2):155–182Google Scholar
  4. Casati R (2017) Two, then four modes of functioning of the mind: Towards a unification of “dual” theories of reasoning and theories of cognitive artifacts. In: Zacks J, Taylor H (eds) Representations in mind and world. Routledge, Oxon and New York, pp 7–23Google Scholar
  5. Celeghin A, de Gelder B, Tamietto M (2015) From affective blindsight to emotional consciousness. Conscious Cogn 36:414–425Google Scholar
  6. Clark JA (2010) Relations of homology between higher cognitive emotions and basic emotions. Biol Philos 25(1):75–94Google Scholar
  7. Clark A, Chalmers D (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):7–19Google Scholar
  8. Damasio AR (1994) Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam Publishing, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  9. Demiris G, Hensel BK (2008) Technologies for an aging society: a systematic review of “smart home” applications. Yearb Med Inform 17(01):33–40Google Scholar
  10. Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot 6(3–4):169–200Google Scholar
  11. Fasoli M (2018) Substitutive, complementary and constitutive cognitive artifacts: developing an interaction-centered approach. Rev Philos Psychol 9(3):671–687Google Scholar
  12. Faucher L, Tappolet C (2006) Introduction: modularity and the nature of emotions. Can J Philos 36(5):vii–xxxiGoogle Scholar
  13. Griffiths PE (1997) What emotions really are: the problem of psychological categories. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  14. Izard CE (2011) Forms and functions of emotions: matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emot Rev 3(4):371–378Google Scholar
  15. Kahneman D (2011) Thinking, fast and slow. MacmillanGoogle Scholar
  16. Krueger J, Szanto T (2016) Extended emotions. Philos Compass 11(12):863–878Google Scholar
  17. Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Feldman MW (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav Brain Sci 23(1):131–146Google Scholar
  18. LeDoux J (1998) The emotional brain: the mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon and Schuster, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. LeDoux J (2012) Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73(4):653–676Google Scholar
  20. Lench HC, Bench SW, Darbor KE, Moore M (2015) A functionalist manifesto: Goal-related emotions from an evolutionary perspective. Emot Rev 7(1):90–98Google Scholar
  21. Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, Barrett LF (2012) The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav Brain sci 35(3):121–143Google Scholar
  22. Norman DA (1991) Cognitive artifacts. Designing Interact Psychol Hum comput Interface 1:17–38Google Scholar
  23. Öhman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108(3):483–522Google Scholar
  24. Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MR, Phelps EA (2005) The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science 309(5735):785–787Google Scholar
  25. Ortony A, Turner TJ (1990) What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychol Rev 97(3):315–331Google Scholar
  26. Panksepp J (1998) Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  27. Pessoa L, Adolphs R (2010) Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a’low road’to’many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci 11(11):773Google Scholar
  28. Piredda G (2019) What is an affective artifact? a further development in situated affectivity. Phenomenol Cogn Sci 1–19Google Scholar
  29. Rozin P, Haidt J, McCauley CR (2008) Disgust. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF (eds). The handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York and London, (pp 757–776)Google Scholar
  30. Scarantino A (2016) The philosophy of emotions and its impact on affective science. In: Barrett LF, Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM (eds) Handbook of emotions, 4th edn. Guilford, New York, pp 3–48Google Scholar
  31. Slaby J (2014) Emotions and the extended mind. In: von Scheve C, Salmela M (eds). Collective emotions. Perspectives from psychology, and sociology. Oxford, Oxford University Press, pp 32–46Google Scholar
  32. Stanovich KE, West RF (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behav Brain Sci 23(5):645–665Google Scholar
  33. Sterelny K (2010) Minds: extended or scaffolded? Phenomenol Cogn Sci 9(4):465–481Google Scholar
  34. Susskind JM, Lee DH, Cusi A, Feima R, Grabski W, Anderson AK (2008) Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat Neurosci 11(7):843–zzzGoogle Scholar
  35. Tooby J, Cosmides L (2008) The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF (eds). The handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York and London, (pp 114–137)Google Scholar
  36. Viola M (under review) Three varieties of emotional artifactsGoogle Scholar
  37. Vytal K, Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: a voxel-based meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 22(12):2864–2885Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Philosophy and EducationUniversity of TurinTurinItaly

Personalised recommendations