Skip to main content

Curriculum and Instruction and the Cognitive Foundations Framework

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Cognitive Foundations of Reading and Its Acquisition

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 20))

  • 1121 Accesses

Abstract

In this chapter we first discuss core instructional programs, focusing on the early elementary school grades where reading is largely taught. We contrast two main types of programs and describe what is known about instructional program effectiveness. We then turn to instructional components. We revisit the work of the National Reading Panel and its study of the evidence underlying certain instructional components, providing an update of its findings and recommendations based on two subsequent reviews that considered more recent research. We also provide maps of the instructional recommendations from each of the three reviews onto the Cognitive Foundations Framework, summarizing the recommendations made and how they address the cognitive components of reading. We then describe some of the specific ways the review panels gave for implementing their recommendations. We follow this with an example of a curriculum map, which takes a one-week instructional sequence from a kindergarten supplemental curriculum and maps it onto the Cognitive Foundations Framework. We close with a discussion of some select issues in curriculum and instruction, a chapter summary, and some questions for further thought.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We use the convention of italicizing key concepts and distinctions for emphasis, but we also italicize words, phrases, or sentences when denoting them as linguistic objects. In both cases, the context we provide should suffice to determine the particular use being served.

  2. 2.

    We follow the standard convention of using arrow brackets (< >) to denote written characters and character combinations, slash brackets (/ /) to denote phonemes and phoneme combinations, and square brackets ([]) to denote distinct speech sounds (phones) and phonetic combinations. To reduce the burden on the reader, we use letters to indicate phonemes and phones rather than the symbols used in the International Phonetic Alphabet, clarifying in text as needed.

  3. 3.

    Strong: weighted mean effect sizes across studies of at least +.20 standard deviations, with combined sample sizes covering at least 500 students or 20 classrooms; Moderate: effect sizes of at least +.20 with combined sample sizes covering 250–499 students or 10–19 classrooms; Limited: effect sizes of +.10 to +.19; and Insufficient: effect sizes less than +.10.

Abbreviations

CAI:

computer-assisted instruction

NRP:

National Reading Panel

PGFS:

Practice Guide for Foundational Skills

PGRC:

Practice Guide for Reading Comprehension

SVR:

Simple View of Reading

References

  • Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1991). Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 789–814). New York, NY: Longman. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203447772.ch28

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2013). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. A. (2011). Efficacy of phonics teaching for reading outcomes: Indications from post-NRP research. In S. A. Brady, D. Braze, & C. A. Fowler (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 69–96). New York, NY: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203817964

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. (2002). Children’s thoughts about reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 199–216. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0602_04

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, J. W., Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (2001). Does success in the Reading Recovery program depend on developing proficiency in phonological processing skills? A longitudinal study in a whole language instructional context. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 141–176. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799xssr0502_2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1993). Reading recovery. Auckland, NZ: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: Phases and findings. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–154). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch8

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ∗Foorman, B. R., Beyler, N., Borradaile, K., Coyne, M., Denton, C. A., Dimino, J., … Wissel, S. (2016). Foundational skills to support reading for understanding in kindergarten through 3rd grade (NCEE 2016-4008). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance (NCEE), Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from the NCEE website. http://whatworks.ed.gov

  • Goodman, K. S. (1967). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. Journal of the Reading Specialist, 6, 126–135. https://doi.org/10.1080/19388076709556976

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. (1983). Context, form and interaction. In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes (pp. 203–211). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-583680-7.50017-5

  • Gough, P. B. (1996, February). A pox on both your houses. Paper presented at the symposium on integrated direct instruction in reading, sponsored by the Language Arts Foundation of America and Oklahoma City Schools, Oklahoma City, OK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 179–196. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02653717

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P. J., Goetz, K., Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., Gibbs, S., & Smith, G. (2006). Evidence for the effectiveness of the early literacy support programme. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709905x39170

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hattie, J. A. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. London, UK: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, Jr., E. D. (2003). Reading comprehension requires knowledge—Of words and the world: Scientific insights into the fourth-grade slump and the nation’s stagnant comprehension scores. American Educator, 27, 10–13, 16–22, 28–29, 48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, R. M., Treiman, R., Carreker, S., & Moats, L. (2008). How words cast their spell: Spelling instruction focused on language, not memory, improves reading and writing. American Educator, 32, 6–16, 42–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1991). Beginning reading. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 759–788). New York, NY: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J., Francis, D. J., & Schatschneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 148–182. https://doi.org/10.1598/rrq.40.2.2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D., Tyner, B., & Perney, J. (2000). Early steps: Replicating the effects of a first-grade reading intervention program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 681–693. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.681

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Assessment of Educational Progress. (2020, January 25). The nation’s report card: Reading. Retrieved from https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/reading/nation/achievement/?grade=4

  • ∗National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • ∗Rose, J. (2006). Independent review of the teaching of early reading: Final Report. Nottingham, UK: Department for Education and Skills.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (2013). The science of reading and its educational implications. Language Learning and Development, 9, 331–360. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.812017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ∗Shanahan, T., Callison, K., Carriere, C., Duke, N. K., Pearson, P. D., Schatschneider, C., & Torgesen, J. (2010). Improving reading comprehension in kindergarten through 3rd grade: A Practice guide (NCEE 2010-4038). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance (NCEE), Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from whatworks.ed.gov/publications/practiceguides

  • Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., Chambers, B., Cheung, A., & Davis, S. (2009). Effective beginning reading programs: A best-evidence synthesis. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University, Center for Data-Driven Reform in Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., Cheung, A., & Davis, S. (2009). Beyond the basics: Effective reading programs for the upper elementary grades. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University, Center for Data-Driven Reform in Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. (1971). Understanding reading. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., & Juel, C. (2005). Teaching children to read: What do we know about how to do it? In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 501–520). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.ch26

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71. https://doi.org/10.2307/747348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 402–433. https://doi.org/10.2307/747605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., Cunningham, A. E., Cipielewski, J., & Siddiqui, S. (1996). The role of inadequate print exposure as a determinant of reading comprehension problems. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention (pp. 15–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Arrow, A. W. (2013). Reading: Phonics instruction. In J. Hattie & E. M. Anderman (Eds.), International guide to student achievement (pp. 316–319). London, UK: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203850398

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2002). The relation of beginning readers’ reported word identification strategies to reading achievement, reading-related skills, and academic self-perceptions. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 341–358. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1015219229515

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2006). Metalinguistic abilities, phonological recoding skills, and the use of sentence context in beginning reading development: A longitudinal study. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 617–635). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2012). Does set for variability mediate the influence of vocabulary knowledge on the development of word recognition skills? Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 122–140. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2010.542527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ∗Tunmer, W. E., & Nicholson, T. (2011). The development and teaching of word recognition skill. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 4, pp. 405–431). New York, NY: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203840412.ch18

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. L. (1999). The American way of spelling: The structure and origins of American English orthography. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hoover, W.A., Tunmer, W.E. (2020). Curriculum and Instruction and the Cognitive Foundations Framework. In: The Cognitive Foundations of Reading and Its Acquisition. Literacy Studies, vol 20. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-44195-1_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-44195-1_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-44194-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-44195-1

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics