Skip to main content

Interspecific Communication: Gaining Information from Heterospecific Alarm Calls

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Animal Signals and Communication ((ANISIGCOM,volume 7))

Abstract

Many birds and mammals give alarm calls when they detect predators or other threats, and these calls have been used as classic models for understanding signal design. Here we consider signal design and usage, and how individuals acquire and use information from the alarm calls of other species. Alarm calls often encode detailed information on danger, such as the type of predator, its current behavior, size, or proximity. Alarm calls are sometimes very similar among species or can share generic acoustic features, and both help to explain recognition of heterospecific alarms. However, alarm calls can vary greatly among species, and taxonomically widespread eavesdropping also requires learning the association between calls and danger. Once heterospecifics eavesdrop on alarm calls, there is potentially selection on callers to modify their alarm calls or usage. If callers benefit from eavesdroppers’ responses to their alarm calls, they may be selected to enhance signal efficacy, leading to interspecific communication and mutual benefit. Alternatively, callers can be selected to manipulate eavesdroppers, using deceptive signaling, including mimicry, causing the eavesdropper to suffer a cost. If callers suffer a cost from eavesdroppers’ responses, their signaling can be modified to make eavesdropping harder, leading to cue denial. Overall, alarm signals provide an insight into the evolution of signal design, and the complex flow of information within and among species in natural communities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adkisson CS, Conner RN (1978) Interspecific vocal imitation in white-eyed vireos. Auk 95:602–606

    Google Scholar 

  • Aubin T (1989) The role of frequency modulation in the process of distress call recognition by the starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Behaviour 108:57–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aubin T (1991) Why do distress calls evoke interspecific responses? An experimental study applied to some species of birds. Behav Process 23:103–111

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aubin T, Brémond JC (1989) Parameters used for recognition of distress calls in two species: Larus argentatus and Sturnus vulgaris. Bioacoustics 2:22–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baylis JR (1982) Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds. Academic, New York, pp 51–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Biedenweg TA, Parsons MH, Fleming PA, Blumstein DT (2011) Sounds scary? Lack of habituation following the presentation of novel sounds. PLoS One 6:e14549

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Billings AC, Greene E, MacArthur-Waltz D (2017) Steller’s jays assess and communicate about predator risk using detection cues and identity. Behav Ecol 28:776–783

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blesdoe EK, Blumstein DT (2014) What is the sound of fear? Behavioral responses of white-crowned sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys to synthesized nonlinear acoustic phenomena. Curr Zool 60:534–541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Chi YY (2012) Scared and less noisy: glucocorticoids are associated with alarm call entropy. Biol Lett 8:189–192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Munos O (2005) Individual, age and sex-specific information is contained in yellow-bellied marmot alarm calls. Anim Behav 69:353–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Récapet C (2009) The sound of arousal: the addition of novel non-linearities increases responsiveness in marmot alarm calls. Ethology 115:1074–1081

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (2011) Principles of animal communication, 2nd edn. Sinaur, Sunderland, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Branch CL, Freeberg TM (2012) Distress calls in tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor): are conspecifics or predators the target? Behav Ecol 23:854–862

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu M (2001a) Heterospecific responses to scream calls and vocal mimicry by Phainopeplas (Phainopepla nitens) in distress. Behaviour 138:775–787

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chu M (2001b) Vocal mimicry in distress calls of Phainopeplas. Condor 103:389–395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K (2006) The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS One 1:e73

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cresswell W (1994) The function of alarm calls in redshanks, Tringa totanus. Anim Behav 47:736–738

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham S, Magrath RD (2017) Functionally referential alarm calls in noisy miners communicate about predator behaviour. Anim Behav 129:171–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curio E, Ernst U, Vieth W (1978) Adaptive significance of avian mobbing II. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds-effectiveness and some constraints. Z Tierpsychol 48:184–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dabelsteen T (2005) Public, private or anonymous? Facilitating and countering eavesdropping. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal communication networks. Cambridge University Press, Melbourne, pp 38–62

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dalziell AH, Magrath RD (2012) Fooling the experts: accurate vocal mimicry in the song of the superb lyrebird, Menura novaehollandiae. Anim Behav 83:1401–1410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalziell AH, Welbergen JA (2016) Elaborate mimetic vocal displays by female Superb lyrebirds. Front Ecol Evol 4:34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalziell AH, Welbergen JA, Igic B, Magrath RD (2015) Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework. Biol Rev 90:643–658

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1872) The expression of emotions in man and animals, University of Chicago Press, 1965 edn. Murray, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson Pell FSE, Potvin DA, Ratnayake CP, Fernández-Juricic E, Magrath RD, Radford AN (2018) Birds orient their heads appropriately in response to functionally referential alarm calls of heterospecifics. Anim Behav 140:109–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vos A, O’Riain J (2010) Sharks shape the geometry of a selfish seal herd: experimental evidence from seal decoys. Biol Lett 6:48–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dooling R (2004) Audition: can birds hear everything they sing? In: Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (eds) Nature’s music: the science of birdsong. Elsevier, San Diego, pp 206–225

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dutour M, Léna J-P, Lengagne T (2017) Mobbing calls: a signal transcending species boundaries. Anim Behav 131:3–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutour M, Léna J-P, Dumet A, Gardette V, Mondy N, Lengagne T (2019) The role of associative learning process on the response of fledgling great tits (Parus major) to mobbing calls. Anim Cogn 22:1095–1103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fallow PM, Magrath RD (2010) Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls. Anim Behav 79:411–417

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallow PM, Gardner JL, Magrath RD (2011) Sound familiar? Acoustic similarity provokes responses to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls. Behav Ecol 22:401–410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallow PM, Pitcher BJ, Magrath RD (2013) Alarming features: birds use specific acoustic properties to identify heterospecific alarm calls. Proc R Soc B 280:20122539

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow LF, Doohan SJ, McDonald PG (2017) Alarm calls of a cooperative bird are referential and elicit context-specific antipredator behavior. Behav Ecol 28:724–731

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feeney WE, Langmore NE (2013) Social learning of a brood parasite by its host. Biol Lett 9:20130443

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ficken MS (1989) Are mobbing calls of Steller’s jays a “confusion chorus”? J Field Ornithol 60:52–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Ficken MS, Popp J (1996) A comparative analysis of passerine mobbing calls. Auk 113:370–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT, Neubauer J, Herzel H (2002) Calls out of chaos: the adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production. Anim Behav 63:407–418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flasskamp A (1994) The adaptive significance of avian mobbing V. An experimental test of the ‘move on’ hypothesis. Ethology 96:322–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower TP (2011) Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc R Soc B 278:1548–1555

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flower TP, Gribble M (2012) Kleptoparasitism by attacks versus false alarm calls in fork-tailed drongos. Anim Behav 83:403–410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower TP, Gribble M, Ridley AR (2014) Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an African bird. Science 344:513–516

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gammon DE, Altizer CE (2011) Northern mockingbirds produce syntactical patterns of vocal mimicry that reflect taxonomy of imitated species. J Field Ornithol 82:158–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghirlanda S, Enquist M (2003) A century of generalization. Anim Behav 66:15–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill SA, Bierema AKM (2013) On the meaning of alarm calls: a review of functional reference in avian alarm calling. Ethology 119:449–461

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill SA, Sealy SG (1996) Nest defence by yellow warblers: recognition of a brood parasite and an avian nest predator. Behaviour 133:263–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2005) Alarm calling in Sri Lankan mixed-species bird flocks. Auk 122:108–120

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2006) Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird. Proc R Soc B 273:875–880

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2008) Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Behav Ecol 19:887–894

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Ratnayake CP, Kotagama SW (2014a) The frequency of vocal mimicry associated with danger varies due to proximity to nest and nesting stage in a passerine bird. Behaviour 151:73–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Ratnayake CP, Kotagama SW (2014b) Vocal mimicry of alarm-associated sounds by a drongo elicits flee and mobbing responses from other species that participate in mixed-species bird flocks. Ethology 120:266–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenlaw JS, Shackelford CE, Brown RE (1998) Call mimicry by eastern towhees and its significance in relation to auditory learning. Wilson Bull 110:431–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69–73

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Learn Behav 32:131–140

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haff TM, Magrath RD (2012) Learning to listen? Nestling response to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 84:1401–1410

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haff TM, Magrath RD (2013) Eavesdropping on the neighbours: fledgling response to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 85:411–418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hailman JP (2009) Context of blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata) mimicking Cooper’s Hawk (Accipiter cooperii) cackle. Fla Field Nat 37:94–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1988) How infant Vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: the role of experience. Behaviour 105:187–201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hetrick SA, Sieving KE (2012) Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information. Behav Ecol 23:83–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarsh AM (1984) Vocal mimicry in starlings. Behaviour 90:302–324

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirth DH, McCullough DR (1977) The evolution of alarm signals in ungulates with special reference to the white-tailed deer. Am Nat 111:31–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollén LI, Radford AN (2009) The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Anim Behav 78:791–800

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurd PL, Wachtmeister C-A, Enquist M (1995) Darwin’s principle of antithesis revisted: a role for perceptual biases in the evolution of intraspecific signals. Proc R Soc B 259:201–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igic B, Magrath RD (2013) Fidelity of vocal mimicry: identification and accuracy of mimicry of heterospecific alarm calls by the brown thornbill. Anim Behav 85:593–603

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igic B, Magrath RD (2014) A songbird mimics different heterospecific alarm calls in response to different types of threat. Behav Ecol 25:538–548

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Igic B, McLachlam J, Lehtinen I, Magrath RD (2015) Crying wolf to a predator: deceptive vocal mimicry by a bird protecting young. Proc R Soc B 282:20150798

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ito R, Mori A (2010) Vigilance against predators induced by eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls in a non-vocal lizard Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguania). Proc R Soc B 277:1275–1280

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito R, Ikeuchi I, Mori A (2013) A day gecko darkens its body color in response to avian alarm calls. Curr Herpetol 32:26–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson FR, McNaughton EJ, Shelly CD, Blumstein DT (2003) Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls. Aust J Zool 51:577–585

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones KJ, Hill WL (2001) Auditory perception of hawks and owls for passerine alarm calls. Ethology 107:717–726

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurisevic MA, Sanderson KJ (1994a) Alarm vocalisations in Australian birds: convergent characteristics and phylogenetic differences. Emu 94:69–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurisevic MA, Sanderson KJ (1994b) The vocal repertoires of six honeyeater (Meliphagidae) species from Adelaide, South Australia. Emu 94:141–148

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurisevic MA, Sanderson KJ (1998) A comparative analysis of distress call structure in Australian passerine and non-passerine species: influence of size and phylogeny. J Avian Biol 29:61–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karp D, Manser M, Wiley EM, Townsend SW (2014) Nonlinearities in meerkat alarm calls prevent receivers from habituating. Ethology 120:189–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley LA, Healy SD (2011) The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus. Naturwissenschaften 98:501–507

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley LA, Coe RL, Madden JR, Healy SD (2008) Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Anim Behav 76:521–528

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) – evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klump GM, Shalter MD (1984) Acoustic behavior of birds and mammals in the predator context. 1. Factors affecting the structure of alarm signals. 2. The functional-significance and evolution of alarm signals. Z Tierpsychol 66:189–226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klump GM, Kretzschmar E, Curio E (1986) The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:317–324

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kostan KM (2002) The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. J Comp Psychol 116:206–209

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T (2002) Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs. Proc R Soc B 269:2345–2350

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T, Igaune K (2006) Mobbing behaviour: reciprocity-based co-operation in breeding pied flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca. Ibis 148:50–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavesley AJ, Magrath RD (2005) Communicating about danger: urgency alarm calling in a bird. Anim Behav 70:365–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Mappes J (1997) Imperfect Batesian mimicry—the effects of the frequency and the distastefulness of the model. Proc R Soc B 264:149–153

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Bennett T (2012) A micro-geography of fear: learning to eavesdrop on alarm calls of neighbouring heterospecifics. Proc R Soc B 279:902–909

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2009a) An avian eavesdropping network: alarm signal reliability and heterospecific response. Behav Ecol 20:745–752

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2009b) Recognition of other species’ aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? Proc R Soc B 276:769–774

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, Horn A, Leonard ML (2010) Calling in the face of danger: predation risk and acoustic communication by parent birds and their offspring. Adv Study Behav 41:187–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015a) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences. Biol Rev 90:560–586

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, McLachlan JR, Igic B (2015b) Wild birds learn to eavesdrop on heterospecific alarm calls. Curr Biol 25:1–4

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc R Soc B 268:2315–2324

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB, Bell MB, Fletcher LB (2001) The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates. Proc R Soc B 268:2485–2491

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2002) Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends Cogn Sci 6:55–57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1955) Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176:6–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1957) Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour 11:13–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall AJ (1950) The function of vocal mimicry in birds. Emu 50:5–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martínez AE, Zenil RT (2012) Foraging guild influences dependence on heterospecific alarm calls in Amazonian bird flocks. Behav Ecol 23:544–550

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mateo JM (1996) Early auditory experience and the ontogeny of alarm-call discrimination in Belding’s ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). J Comp Psychol 110:115–124

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1965) The evolution of alarm calls. Am Nat 99:59–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor PK, Dabelsteen T (1996) Communication networks. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University press, Ithaca, pp 409–425

    Google Scholar 

  • McLachlan JR, Ratnayake CP, Magrath RD (2019) Personal information about danger trumps social information from avian alarm calls. Proc R Soc B 286:20182945

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McLachlan JR, Magrath RD (2020) Speedy revelations: how alarm calls can convey rapid, reliable information about urgent danger. Proc R Soc B. (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Meise K, Franks DW, Bro-Jørgensen J (2018) Multiple adaptive and non-adaptive processes determine responsiveness to heterospecific alarm calls in African savannah herbivores. Proc R Soc B 285:20172676

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Møller AP (1988) False alarm calls as a means of resource usurpation in the great tit Parus major. Ethology 79:25–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1976) Vocal mimicry in the thick-billed Euphonia. Wilson Bull 88:485–486

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1977) On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. Am Nat 111:855–869

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (2017) Animal vocal communication: assessment and management roles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Müller CA, Manser MB (2008) The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms. Anim Behav 75:897–904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munn CA (1986) Birds that ‘cry wolf’. Nature 319:143–145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray TG, Magrath RD (2015) Does signal deterioration compromise eavesdropping on other species’ alarm calls? Anim Behav 108:33–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M, Wiley RH (2001) Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication. Anim Behav 62:825–837

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naguib M, Mundry R, Ostreiher R, Hultsch H, Schrader L, Todt D (1999) Cooperatively breeding Arabian babblers call differently when mobbing in different predator-induced situations. Behav Ecol 10:636–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neudorf DL, Sealy SG (2002) Distress calls of birds in a neotropical cloud forest. Biotropica 34:118–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nocera JJ, Ratcliffe LM (2010) Migrant and resident birds adjust antipredator behavior in response to social information accuracy. Behav Ecol 21:121–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oatley T (1969) The functions of vocal imitation by African Cossyphas. Ostrich 40:85–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oda R, Masataka N (1996) Interspecific responses of ringtailed lemurs to playback of antipredator alarm calls given by Verreaux’s sifakas. Ethology 102:441–453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parejo D, Avilés JM, Rodríguez J (2012) Alarm calls modulate the spatial structure of a breeding owl community. Proc R Soc B 279:2135–2141

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Peake TM (2005) Eavesdropping in communication networks. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal communication networks. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 13–37

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pettifor RA (1990) The effects of avian mobbing on a potential predator, the European kestrel, Falco tinnunculus. Anim Behav 39:821–827

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potvin DA, Ratnayake CP, Radford AN, Magrath RD (2018) Birds learn socially to recognize heterospecific alarm calls by acoustic association. Curr Biol 28:2632–2637

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radford AN, Bell MBV, Hollen LI, Ridley AR (2011) Singing for your supper: sentinel calling by kleptoparasites can mitigate the cost to victims. Evolution 65:900–906

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rainey HJ, Züberbuhler K, Slater PJB (2004) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc B 271:755–759

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ramakrishnan U, Coss RG (2000) Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalizations by bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). J Comp Psychol 114:3–12

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randler C, Förschler MI (2011) Heterospecifics do not respond to subtle differences in chaffinch mobbing calls: message is encoded in number of elements. Anim Behav 82:725–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratnayake CP, Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2010) Two sympatric species of passerine birds imitate the same raptor calls in alarm contexts. Naturwissenchaften 97:103–108

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rendall D, Owren MJ, Ryan MJ (2009) What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 78:233–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley AR, Child MF, Bell MBV (2007) Interspecific audience effects on the alarm-calling behaviour of a kleptoparasitic bird. Biol Lett 3:589–591

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley AR, Wiley EM, Thompson AM (2014) The ecological benefits of interceptive eavesdropping. Funct Ecol 28:197–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riegert J, Jůzlová Z (2018) Vocal mimicry in the song of Icterine warblers (Hippolais icterina): possible functions and sources of variability. Ethol Ecol Evol 30:430–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rooke IJ, Knight TA (1977) Alarm calls of honeyeaters with reference to locating sources of sound. Emu 77:193–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rydén O (1978) The significance of antecedent auditory experiences on later reactions to the ‘seeet’ alarm-call in great tit nestlings Parus major. Z Tierpsychol 47:396–409

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rydén O (1982) Selective resistance to approach: a precursor to fear responses to an alarm call in great tit nestlings Parus major. Dev Psychobiol 15:113–120

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt KA, Dall SRX, van Gils JA (2010) The ecology of information: an overview on the ecological significance of making informed decisions. Oikos 119:304–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication: reliability and deception in signalling systems. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Yasukawa K (2017) Eavesdropping and cue denial in avian acoustic signals. Anim Behav 124:273–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seppänen A, Forsman JT (2007) Interspecific social learning: novel preference can be acquired from a competing species. Curr Biol 17:1248–1252

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seppänen JT, Forsman JT, Monkkonen M, Thomson RL (2007) Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. Ecology 88:1622–1633

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980a) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801–803

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980b) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman PW (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science 197:1246–1253

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman PW (1985) Alarm calls of Belding’s ground squirrels to aerial predators: nepotism or self-preservation? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 17:313–323

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shriner WM (1999) Antipredator responses to a previously neutral sound by free-living adult golden-mantled ground squirrels, Spermophilus lateralis (Sciuridae). Ethology 105:747–757

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter EI, Berlin ER, Bower JT, Blumstein DT (2013) A test of the nonlinearity hypothesis in great-tailed grackles (Quiscalus mexicanus). Ethology 119:309–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow BK (1974) Vocal mimicry in violaceous Euphonia, Euphonia violacea. Wilson Bull 86:179–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Sridhar H, Beauchamp G, Shanker K (2009) Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis. Anim Behav 78:337–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan U, Raza RH, Quadar S (2010) The nuclear question: rethinking species importance in multi-species animal groups. J Anim Ecol 79:948–954

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan KA (1985) Selective alarm calling by downy woodpeckers in mixed-species flocks. Auk 102:184–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2011) Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats. Curr Biol 21:R15–R16

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2016) Semantic communication in birds: evidence from field research over the past two decades. Ecol Res 31:307–319

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN, Wheatcroft D, Griesser M (2016) Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nat Commun 7:10986

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tegtman N, Magrath RD (in press) Discriminating between similar alarm calls of contrasting function. Philos Trans R Soc B.

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E (2007) Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:5479–5482

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ten Cate C, Rowe C (2007) Biases in signal evolution: learning makes a difference. Trends Ecol Evol 22:380–387

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend SW, Manser M (2011) The function of nonlinear phenomena in meerkat alarm calls. Biol Lett 7:47–49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend SW, Manser MB (2013) Functionally referential communication in mammals: the past, present and the future. Ethology 119:1–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon CJ (1973) Vocal imitation by South African birds. Ostrich 44:23–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vieth W, Curio E, Ernst U (1980) The adaptive significance of avian mobbing III. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds - cross-species tutoring and properties of learning. Anim Behav 28:1217–1229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, St Clair JJH (2007) Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biol Lett 3:632–634

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatcroft D (2015) Repetition rate of calls used in multiple contexts communicates presence of predators to nestlings and adult bird. Anim Behav 103:35–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatcroft D, Price T (2013) Learning and signal copying facilitate communication among bird species. Proc R Soc B 280:20123070

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wheatcroft D, Price TD (2015) Rates of signal evolution are associated with the nature of interspecific communication. Behav Ecol 26:83–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH (2015) Noise matters: the evolution of communication. Harvard, Cambridge, MA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH, Richard DG (1982) Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Ouellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, Production, perception, and design features of sound, vol 1. Academic, New York, pp 131–181

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wise KK, Conover MR, Knowlton FF (1999) Response of coyotes to avian distress calls: testing the startle-predator and predator-attraction hypotheses. Behaviour 136:935–949

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zann R, Dunstan E (2008) Mimetic song in superb lyrebirds: species mimicked and mimetic accuracy in different populations and age classes. Anim Behav 76:1043–1054

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zollinger SA, Suthers RA (2004) Motor mechanisms of a vocal mimic: implications for birdsong production. Proc R Soc B 271:483–491

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000a) Causal knowledge of predators’ behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Anim Behav 59:209–220

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000b) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc B 267:713–718

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2009) Survivor signals: the biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. Adv Study Behav 40:277–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Noë R, Seyfarth RM (1997) Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Anim Behav 53:589–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank all our collaborators in our work on communication and eavesdropping in Australian birds. In recent years these have included Lauren Ascah, Tom Bennett, Andrew Cockburn, Sean Cunningham, Francesca Dawson Pell, Anastasia Dalziell, Pam Fallow, Janet Gardner, Esteban Fernández Juricic, Jessica McLachlan, Trevor Murray, Helen Osmond, Dominique Potvin, Andy Radford, Chaminda Ratnayake, Tom Rowell, Natalie Tegtman, You Zhou, and others who have helped in various ways. We are also grateful for financial support from the Australian Research Council and the Research School of Biology, ANU, and to the Australian Bird and Bat Banding Scheme, Australian National Botanic Gardens, and ANU Ethics Committee permission to carry out the work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert D. Magrath .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Magrath, R.D., Haff, T.M., Igic, B. (2020). Interspecific Communication: Gaining Information from Heterospecific Alarm Calls. In: Aubin, T., Mathevon, N. (eds) Coding Strategies in Vertebrate Acoustic Communication. Animal Signals and Communication, vol 7. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-39200-0_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics