Skip to main content

Empathy as Spontaneous Communication: At the Intersection of the Traditional Social and Behavioral Sciences and the New Affective and Communication Sciences

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Social Intelligence and Nonverbal Communication
  • 2402 Accesses

Abstract

Humans have within an ongoing symphony of feelings and desires functioning as voices of the genes. Genes also broadcast their voices outside the individual organism to the world via the displays and preattunements of spontaneous communication. In this chapter, we first consider subjectively experienced affect, the roles of the individual expressivity of senders and accuracy of receivers in communication, and dyad-level accuracy in unique sender-receiver pairings. Second, we consider the development of empathy, dyad-level variables such as interpersonal immediacy and synchrony, selfish gene theory, and expressivity as a marker of honest signaling and trustworthiness. We then consider emotion and empathy in media communication including the uncanny valley, trust in machines, empathy in entertainment including silly love songs and motion pictures, and the “empathy molecule” oxytocin.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Many suggest that higher-level emotions must involve cognitive appraisal, but the presence of higher-level emotions among animals and young children excludes this possibility (e.g., Bloom & Wynn, 2016; de Waal & Preston, 2017).

  2. 2.

    See Buck, Graham, Allred, and Hancock, this volume.

  3. 3.

    From the Rogers and Hammerstein musical South Pacific.

  4. 4.

    © 1980 by the J. Geils Band.

References

  • Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28(3), 289–304. https://doi.org/10.2307/2786027

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., & Oleson, K. C. (1991). Current status of the empathy-altruism hypothesis. In M. Clark (Ed.), Altruism. Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 62–85). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P., & Wynn, K. (2016). What develops in moral development? In D. Barner & A. S. Baron (Eds.), Oxford series in cognitive development. Core knowledge and conceptual change (pp. 347–364). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190467630.003.0019

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional expressivity and trustworthiness: The role of nonverbal behavior in the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, R. T., & Buck, R. (2004). Emotion receiving ability: A new view of measuring individual differences in the ability to accurately judge others’ emotions. In G. Geher (Ed.), Measuring emotional intelligence: Common ground and controversy (pp. 73–89). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braten, S., & Trevarthen, C. (2007). From infant intersubjectivity and participant movements to simulation and conversation in cultural common sense. In S. Braten (Ed.), On being moved: From mirror neurons to empathy (pp. 21–33). Amsterdam: The Netherlands, John Benjamins. ISBN-13: 978-9027252043.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Braun, M. F., & Bryan, A. (2006). Female waist-to-hip and male waist-to-shoulder ratios as determinants of romantic partner desirability. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 805–819. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407506068264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1983). Emotional development and emotional education. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotions in early development. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 389–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1988). Human motivation and emotion (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1990). Rapport, emotional education, and emotional competence. Psychological Inquiry, 1(4), 301–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (2010). Retrieved August 1, 2010, from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/spontaneous-emotion/201008/silly-love-songs

  • Buck, R. (2011). Communicative genes in the evolution of empathy and altruism. Behavioral Genetics, 41, 876–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1991). Emotional communication and altruism: The communicative gene hypothesis. In M. Clark (Ed.), Altruism. Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 149–175). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Ginsburg, B. (1997). Communicative genes and the evolution of empathy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 17–43). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., Khan, M., Fagan, M., & Coman, E. (2018). The user affective experience scale: A measure of emotions anticipated in response to pop-up warnings. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies., 24(1), 25–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2017.1314612

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Powers, S. R. (2006). The biological foundations of social organization: The dynamic emergence of social structure through nonverbal communication. In V. Manusov & M. Patterson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 119–138). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R., & Van Lear, C. A. (2002). Verbal and nonverbal communication: Distinguishing symbolic, spontaneous, and pseudo-spontaneous nonverbal behavior. Journal of Communication, 52, 522–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S., & Winkielman, P. (2012). Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: How does it work? What does it mean? Hormones and Behavior, 61(3), 392399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2005). Forebrain emotional asymmetry: A neuroanatomical basis? Trends in Cognitive Sciences., 9, 566–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press. [See also 1989 edition, p. 33].

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1994). Burying the vehicle. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 616–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K. W. (2012). Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: An integrative review and research agenda. Hormones and Behavior, 61(3), 419–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). The ‘Russian doll’ model of empathy and imitation. In S. Braten (Ed.), On being moved: From mirror neurons to empathy (pp. 35–48). Amsterdam: The Netherlands, John Benjamins. ISBN-13: 978-9027252043.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & Preston, S. D. (2017). Mammalian empathy: Behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 498–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, S. l., Dal Monte, O., Noble, P., & Averbeck, B. B. (2014). Intranasal oxytocin effects of social cognition: A critique. Brain Research, 11, 69–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 61(3), 380–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York, NY: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. (2001). Cooperation through emotional commitment. In R. M. Hesse (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 57–76). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1971). Learning to love. San Francisco: Albion.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884/1968). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188–205. Reprinted in M. Arnold (Ed.), The Nature of Emotion (pp. 188–205). Baltimore, MD: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A. (in press). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khan, M. M. H., Buck, R., Coman, E., Albayram, Y., Jensen, T., & Fagan, M. (2017, June 28). Role of effective communication in trust building: Application to human-computer interaction. Presented at the 2017 Trust and Influence Program Review. Air Force Office of Scientific Research (AFOSR), Colorado Springs, Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (2nd ed., pp. 380–402). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S. R., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. D., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7, 730–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lallemand, C., Gronier, G., & Koenig, V. (2015). User experience: A concept without consensus? Exploring practitioners’ perspectives through an international survey. Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 35–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welsh, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1966). Evolution of ritualization in the biological and cultural spheres. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Britain, 251, 273–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loumaye, E., Thorner, J., & Catt, K. J. (1982). Yeast mating pheromone activates mammalian gonadotropins: Evolutionary conservation of a reproductive hormone? Science, 218(4579), 1323–1325. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.6293058

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markov, P. O. (1950). A study of interoception in human subjects. Uch. Zap. Leningr. U. Sem. Biol., 22, 345–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLuhan, M. (1969). Counterblast. Toronto, ON: McClelland & Stewart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, M. (2012). The uncanny valley (K. F. MacDorman & N. Kageki, Trans.). IEEE Robotics and Automation. 19. 98–100. https://doi.org/10.1109/MRA.2012.2192811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20, 750–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, I. D. (2008). Brain oxytocin: A key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 20(6), 858–865. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01726.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papez, J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 38, 725–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pert, C. (1997). Molecules of emotion: Why you feel the way you feel. New York, NY: Scribner’s.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 19(2), 225–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(94)00066-A

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. (1961). The observable unconscious and the inferable conscious in current Soviet psychophysiology. Psychological Review, 68, 81–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, L. I., Zeglen, K. N., & Schmidt, K. L. (2012). Facial expressions as honest signals of cooperative intent in a one-shot anonymous prisoner’s dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 200–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1903). The principles of mathematics. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabatelli, R., Buck, R., & Dreyer, A. (1982). Nonverbal communication accuracy in married couples: Relationships with marital complaints. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1088–1097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabatelli, R., Buck, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). Nonverbal communication in married couples: A social relations analysis. Journal of Personality, 54, 513–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schug, J., Matsumoto, D., Horita, Y., Yamagishi, T., & Bonnet, K. (2010). Emotional expressivity as a signal of cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 87–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Fischer, M., Dvash, J., Harari, H., Perach-Bloom, N., & Levkovitz, Y. (2009). Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases envy and schadenfreude (gloating). Biological Psychiatry, 66(9), 864–870. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strassmann, J. E., Zhu, Y., & Queller, D. C. (2000). Altruism and social cheating in the social amoeba Dictyostelium discoideum. Nature, 408, 965–967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stifano, S. (2014). Belief. https://www.imdb.com/title/tt1773304

  • Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285–293. http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=1047-840X%281990%291%3A4%3C285%3ATNORAI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-T

  • Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived activities: Their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 27, 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. M., Kenny, D. A., & Stoto, M. (1979). A new round robin analysis of variance for social interaction data. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 37(10), 1742–1757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, C. M., & Bassler, B. L. (2005). Quorum sensing: Cell-to-cell communication in bacteria. Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 21, 319–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., & Sober, E. (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 585–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1977). The costs of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principal). Journal of Theoretical Biology, 67, 603–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ross Buck .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Buck, R., Stifano, S., Graham, B., Allred, R.J. (2020). Empathy as Spontaneous Communication: At the Intersection of the Traditional Social and Behavioral Sciences and the New Affective and Communication Sciences. In: Sternberg, R.J., Kostić, A. (eds) Social Intelligence and Nonverbal Communication. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-34964-6_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics