Skip to main content

Role of Dialogue in Public Opinion Formation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover The Origin of Dialogue in the News Media

Part of the book series: Challenges to Democracy in the 21st Century ((CDC))

  • 314 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter shows that the frames present in the dialogue in the news media are highly relevant for the vote outcome. We show a framing effect in two of the three cases. These frames remain important for the voting decision when controlling for and in comparison with partisan heuristic. We can state that the more important the topic for a person, the less polarized the context, and the less complex the topic, the more a person relies on the frame-based path (thus, on arguments and less on heuristics like partisan orientation or social identities in general). In this regard, the chapter gives empirical support to the realistic theory of democracy. We do not expect arguments to play a more important role in other types of campaigns or other contexts. What we find here is the most that can be expected from citizens.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The exact question was: “Which of the ten arguments from the list (shown to the campaigner) do you conceive as the three most important, as the single most important, and the three least important?” I do not use the evaluation of the most and the three most important arguments here.

  2. 2.

    The specific questionnaire item reads as follows (for the humanitarian tradition frame , for example): “On a scale from 0 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree), how much do you agree with the argument: The humanitarian tradition must be maintained?”

  3. 3.

    For the vote intentions, the specific questionnaire item reads as follows: “If there were a ballot tomorrow, would you be strongly in favor, rather in favor, rather against or strongly against the toughening of the asylum law?” Being (strongly or rather) in favor of the proposition is coded as 1, and being undecided or (rather or strongly) against as 0. For the vote choice, the questionnaire item reads: “How did you vote? Did you agree with or reject the asylum law?” Again, agreement is coded as 1 and non-decision and rejection as 0.

  4. 4.

    Respondents who do not attribute a utility higher than five to any of the six parties, plus the (very few) respondents who attribute equal preference to all of the parties, are considered not to have any particular partisan predisposition and, accordingly, are dropped from the analysis. I have used MUDFOLD 4.0 for my analysis (see, Van Schuur and Post 1998).

  5. 5.

    The five statements concerned individual safety threat (“I am afraid of increasing violence and vandalism in my neighborhood by foreigners”), individual economic threat (“I am afraid that my economic prospects will get worse because of foreigners”), collective safety threat (“I am afraid of increasing violence and vandalism in Swiss society by foreigners”), collective cultural threat (“These days, I am afraid that the Swiss culture is threatened by foreigners”), and collective economic threat (“I am afraid that the economic prospects of Swiss society will get worse because of foreigners”). With regard to partisan predispositions, these questions were only asked once, at the beginning of the campaign, since my aim is to determine the extent to which voters converge on their pre-campaign issue-specific predispositions.

  6. 6.

    The questions were part of a battery that asked for the kind of country the respondents preferred. Each item asked the recipient to choose between polar contrasts: “Please tell me what kind of Switzerland you prefer: a Switzerland… a) …with more state interventions in the economy, or with more market competition; b)…that places more emphasis on solidarity, or on individual responsibility; c)…with large income differences, or small income differences?” Each item was measured on a five-point scale, ranging from strong agreement with the first part to strong agreement with the second part of the item.

  7. 7.

    The most common measures used for ambivalence are the multiplicative measure used here (see Keele and Wolak 2008: 680), and Griffin’s ambivalence index (or a modified version of this index) (see Steenbergen and Brewer 2004: 103f.).

  8. 8.

    The exact question was (example for the humanitarian tradition frame ): “There have been different opinions with regard to the asylum law. In the following, I will read aloud some arguments. Could you please indicate with ‘yes’, ‘no’, or ‘don’t know’ whether you have ever heard of them in the current debate: ‘The humanitarian tradition must be maintained’?”

  9. 9.

    These illustrations are based on the model used for Table A.6. I used the variables relevant for the specific time point and set the other variables at their means.

  10. 10.

    The point estimates for the linear combination for the difference in frame strength between two time points provide the following results:

    Human. trad. frame: (t2)−(t1): z = −3.24, P > z = 0.001, (t3)−(t1): z = −3.72, P > z = 0.000, (t3)−(t2): z = −0.98, P > z = 0.329.

    Abuse frame : (t2)−(t1): z = 1.76, P > z = 0.078, (t3)−(t1): z = 2.64, P > z = 0.008, (t3)−(t2): 1.03, P > z = 0.301.

    Efficacy frame : (t2)−(t1): 2.38, P > z=0.017, (t3)−(t1): z = 3.32, P > z = 0.001, (t3)−(t2): 1.24, P > z = 0.215.

  11. 11.

    SME frame : (t2)−(t1): z = 2.3, P > z = 0.022.

  12. 12.

    Tax equity frame : (t2)−(t1): z = 4.35, P > z = 0.000.

  13. 13.

    All campaign arguments were jointly included in one factor analysis.

  14. 14.

    Based on the partisan predisposition scores, I coded left as including Green and Social Democrat voters, moderate right as in-between voters favoring Christian Democrats and Liberals, and conservative right as including voters favoring the People’s Party.

  15. 15.

    The predicted probabilities are calculated for decided voters at the end of the campaign with mean values on remaining variables.

References

  • Achen, C. H. & Bartels, L. M. (2016). Democracy for Realists. Why Elections do not Produce Responsive Government. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez, R. M., & Brehm, J. (1995). American Ambivalence Towards Abortion Policy: Development of a Heteroscedastic Probit Model of Competing Values. American Journal of Political Science, 39, 1055–1082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez, R. M., & Brehm, J. (2002). Hard Chopices, Easy Answers. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudreau, C., & MacKenzie, S. A. (2014). Informing the Electorate? How Party Cues and Policy Information Affect Public Opinion About Initiatives. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 48–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, S. (2015). Information Availability and Information Use in Ballot Proposition Contests: Are Voters Over-Burdened?*. Electoral Studies, 38, 183–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, S., & Donovan, T. (1998). Demanding Choices: Opinion, Voting, and Direct Democracy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody, R. A., & Page, B. I. (1972). Comment-Assessment of Policy Voting. American Journal of Political Science, 66(2), 450–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, J. G. (2011). Elite Influence on Public Opinion in an Informed Electorate. American Political Science Review, 105(August), 496–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A., Converse, P., Miller, W., & Stokes, D. (1960). The American Voter. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., & Trop, Y. (1999). Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). A Theory of Framing and Opinion Formation in Competitive Elite Environments. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 99–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). Framing Theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ciuk, D. J., & Yost, B. A. (2016). The Effects of Issue Salience, Elite Influence, and Policy Content on Public Opinion. Political Communication, 33(2), 328–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party Over Policy: The Dominating Impact of Group Influence on Political Beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808–822.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, C., & Kriesi, H. (2017). Party, Policy – or Both? Partisan Biased Processing of Policy Arguments in Direct Democracy. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 27(3), 235–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coombs, C. H. (1964). A Theory of Data. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2001). The Implications of Framing Effects for Citizen Competence. Political Behavior, 23, 225–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N. (2009). What’s It All About? Framing in Political Science. In G. Keren (Ed.), Perspectives on Framing. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman, J. N., & Nelson, K. R. (2003). Framing and Deliberation: How Citizens’ Conversations Limit Elite Influence. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 729–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunaway, J., & Lawrence, R. G. (2015). What Predicts the Game Frame? Media Ownership, Electoral Context, and Campaign News. Political Communication, 32(1), 43–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engeli, I., Anouk, L., & Nai, A. (2008). Analysis of the Federal Votes of June 1, 2008 (Analyse der eidgenössischen Abstimmungen vom 1. Juni 2008). Vox Analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame Analysis. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golder, L. (2016). Keine Angst vor Emotionen: in der direkten Demokratie überwiegen Argumente. Bern: Gfs Bern. http://www.gfsbern.ch/de-ch/Detail/keine-angst-vor-emotionen-in-der-direkten-demokratie-ueberwiegen-argumente. September 2017.

  • Hänggli, R., & Kriesi, H. (2010). Political Framing Strategies and Their Impact on Media Framing in a Swiss Direct-Democratic Campaign. Political Communication, 27(2), 141–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirter, H., & Linder, W. (2008). Analysis of the Federal Votes of February 24, 2008 (Analyse der eidgenössischen Abstimmungen vom 24. Februar 2008). Vox Analysis. Berne: University of Berne.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobolt, S. B. (2016). The Brexit Vote: A Divided Nation, a Divided Continent. Journal of European Public Policy, 23(9), 1259–1277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobolt, S., & Brouard, S. (2011). Contesting the European Union? Why the Dutch and the French Rejected the European Constitution. Political Research Quarterly, 64(2), 309–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News That Matters: Television and American Opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, W. G. (1988). The Impact of Party Identification on Issue Attitudes. American Journal of Political Science, 32(3), 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, L., & Wolak, J. (2008). Contextual Sources of Ambivalence. Political Psychology, 29(5), 653–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1990). Mimicking Political Debate with Survey Questions: The Case of White Opinion on Affirmative Action for Blacks. Social Cognition, 8, 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kriesi, H. (2005). Direct Democratic Choice: The Swiss Experience. Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kriesi, H., & Trechsel, A. H. (2008). The Politics of Switzerland. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kühne, R., Schemer, C., Matthes, J., & Wirth, W. (2011). Affective Priming in Political Campaigns: How Campaign-Induced Emotions Prime Political Opinions. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 23(4), 485–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenz, G. S. (2009). Learning and Opinion Change, Not Priming: Reconsidering the Priming Hypothesis. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 821–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenz, G. (2012). Follow the Leader? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., & Hamill, R. (1986). A Partisan Schema for Political Information Processing. The American Political Science Review, 80(2), 505–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupia, A. (2016). Uninformed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maatsch, S. (2007). The Struggle to Control Meanings: The French Debate on the European Constitution in the Mass Media. Perspectives on European Politics and Society, 8(3), 261–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mettler, S. (2018). The Government-Citizen Disconnect. Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997). Toward a Psychology of Framing Effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Skrondal, A. (2005). Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Stata (2nd ed.). College Station, TX: Stata Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, W. M. (1993). The Role of Partisan Stereotypes in Information Processing About Political Candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 472–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, T. J. (2005). Group Attachment and the Reduction of Value-Driven Ambivalence. Political Psychology, 26, 905–928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seeberg, H. B., Slothuus, R., & Stubager, R. (2017). Do Voters Learn? Evidence that Voters Respond Accurately to Changes in Political Parties’ Policy Positions. West European Politics, 40(2), 336–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selb, P., Kriesi, H., Hänggli, R., & Marr, M. (2009). Partisan Choices in a Direct-Democratic Campaign. European Political Science Review, 1(1), 155–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, F., & Edwards, H. H. (2005). Economic Individualism, Humanitarianism, and Welfare Reform: A Value-Based Account of Framing Effects. Journal of Communication, 55(4), 795–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slothuus, R. (2008). More Than Weighting Cognitive Importance: A Dual-Process Model of Issue Framing Effects. Political Psychology, 29(1), 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slothuus, R. (2010). When Can Political Parties Lead Public Opinion? Evidence from a Natural Experiment. Political Communication, 27(2), 158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sniderman, P. M., & Theriault, S. M. (2004). The Structure of Political Argument and the Logic of Issue Framing. In P. M. Sniderman & S. M. Theriault (Eds.), Studies in Public Opinion: Attitudes, Nonattitudes, Measurement Error and Change (pp. 133–165). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Steenbergen, M., & Brewer, P. R. (2004). The Not-So-Ambivalent Public: Policy Attitudes in the Political Culture of Ambivalence. In W. E. Saris & P. M. Sniderman (Eds.), Studies in Public Opinion. Attitudes, Nonattitudes, Measurement Errors, and Change (pp. 93–132). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tillie, J. (1995). Party Utility and Voting Behavior. Amsterdam: Het Spinhuis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Eijk, C., Van der Brug, W. W., Kroh, M., & Franklin, M. (2006). Rethinking the Dependent Variable in Voting Behavior: On the Measurement and Analysis of Electoral Utilities. Electoral Studies, 25(3), 424–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Schuur, W. H. (1993). Nonparametric Unfolding Models for Multicategory Data. Political Analysis, 4, 41–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Schuur, W. H., & Post, W. J. (1998). MUDFOLD: A Program for Multiple Unidimensional Unfolding. Version 4.0 [Software manual]. Groningen: ProGAMMA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, W., Schemer, C., Kühne, R., & Matthes, J. (2010). The Impact of Positive and Negative Affects in Direct-Democratic Campaigns. In H. Kriesi (Ed.), Manipulation or Deliberation. Zurich: Unpublished Manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Regula Hänggli .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hänggli, R. (2020). Role of Dialogue in Public Opinion Formation. In: The Origin of Dialogue in the News Media. Challenges to Democracy in the 21st Century. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26582-3_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics