Skip to main content

Introduction: Psychological Studies of Science and Technology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Psychological Studies of Science and Technology

Abstract

Science and technology are central to almost all domains of human activity. The study of science and technology is evident in subdisciplines such as philosophy of science, philosophy of technology, sociology of knowledge, and history of science and technology, and in interdisciplines such as Science and Technology Studies. To date, psychology has been marginal in this space. We argue that this is in part owing to psychology viewing itself as a science (and not an object of study). It is also a result of previous attempts toward developing a psychology of science and technology explicitly adopting a narrow epistemological stance that is at odds with the broader epistemic foundation of Science and Technology Studies and related fields.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adorno, T., & Horkheimer, M. (2002). Dialectic of enlightenment. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press (Originally published 1944).

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, H. (2016). Collaboration, interdisciplinarity, and the epistemology of contemporary science. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A,56, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bachelard, G. (2002). The formation of the scientific mind: A contribution to a psychoanalysis of objective knowledge. Manchester, UK: Clinamen Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, F. (1937). Novum organum (R. F. Jones, Ed.). New York: Odyssey Press (Originally published 1620).

    Google Scholar 

  • Barad, K. (1996). Meeting the universe halfway: Realism and social constructivism without contradiction. In L. H. Nelson & J. Nelson (Eds.), Feminism, science and the philosophy of science (pp. 161–194). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Blackman, L., Cromby, J., Hook, D., Papadopoulos, D., & Walkerdine, V. (2008). Creating subjectivities. Subjectivity,22(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloor, D. (1975). A philosophical approach to science. Social Studies of Science,5(4), 507–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (1995). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (A. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell, & L. McAlister, Trans.). London: Routledge (Originally published 1874).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1949). On the perception of incongruity: A paradigm. Journal of Personality,18(2), 206–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burman, E. (2016). Deconstructing developmental psychology. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1989). Fragments of the fragile history of psychological epistemology and theory of science. In B. Gholson, W. R. Shadish, Jr., R. A. Neimeyer, & A. C. Houts (Eds.), Psychology of science: Contributions to metascience (pp. 21–46). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Choynowski, M. (1948, September). Life of science. Synthese,6(5–6), 248–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall, A., & Dreier, O. (Eds.). (2006). Doing things with things: The design and use of everyday objects. Aldershot: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1994). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, H. (2009). Science, policy, and the value-free ideal. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dreier, O. (2008). Psychotherapy in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreier, O. (2016). Conduct of everyday life: Implications for critical psychology. In E. Schraube & C. Højholt (Eds.), Psychology and the conduct of everyday life (pp. 15–33). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H. (1987). John M. MacEachran memorial lecture series; 1985. Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eiduson, B. T. (1962). Scientists: Their psychological world. Oxford, UK: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fancher, R. E. (1983). Alphonse de Candolle, Francis Galton, and the early history of the nature-nurture controversy. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences,19(4), 341–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fanon, F. (1967). Black skin, white masks. New York: Grove Press (Originally published 1952).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J., & Gorman, M. E. (1998). The psychology of science: Review and integration of a nascent discipline. Review of General Psychology,2(1), 3–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J., & Gorman, M. E. (Eds.). (2013). Handbook of the psychology of science. New York: Springer Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against method. London: Verso Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1966). Les Mots et les Choses. Paris: Éditions Gallimard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1975). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox-Keller, E. (1985). Reflections on gender and science. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, S. (2002). Social epistemology. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Stephens, A. L. (1983). Mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26(2), 309–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. (2000). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (Ed.). (1992). Cognitive models of science (Vol. 15). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (1985). In nature’s school: Faraday as a natural philosopher. In D. Gooding & F. James (Eds.), Faraday rediscovered (pp. 105–135). London: Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gordo-López, Á. J., & Parker, I. (Eds.). (1999). Cyberpsychology. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorman, M., & Carlson, W. (1990). Interpreting invention as a cognitive process: The case of Alexander Graham Bell, Thomas Edison, and the telephone. Science, Technology and Human Values,15, 131–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, R. V. (2004). Even the rat was white: A historical view of psychology (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. (1985). A manifesto for cyborgs: Science, technology and socialist feminism in the 1980s. Socialist Review,80, 65–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harding, S. (1986). The science question in feminism. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R. (1979). Social being: A theory for social psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven psychologies. London, UK: Century/Random House UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (2013). Psychology: Social self-understanding on the reasons for action in the conduct of everyday life. In E. Schraube & U. Osterkamp (Eds.), Psychology from the standpoint of the subject: Selected writings of Klaus Holzkamp (A. Boreham & U. Osterkamp, Trans.) (pp. 233–341). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (2016). Conduct of everyday life as a basic concept of critical psychology. In E. Schraube & C. Højholt (Eds.), Psychology and the conduct of everyday life (pp. 65–98). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houts, A. C. (1989). Contributions of the psychology of science to metascience: A call for explorers. In B. Gholson, W. R. Shadish, Jr., R. A. Neimeyer, & A. C. Houts (Eds.), Psychology of science: Contributions to metascience (pp. 47–88). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1954). The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology (D. Carr, Trans.). Evanston: Northwestern University Press (Originally published 1936).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ichikawa, J. J. (2012). Experimentalist pressure against traditional methodology. Philosophical Psychology,25(5), 743–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1987). Pragmatism. In William James: Writings 1902–1910 (pp. 479–624). New York: Library of America (Originally published 1907).

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs (Vols. 1–2). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr-Cetina, K. (1981). The manufacture of knowledge: An essay on the constructivist and contextual nature of science. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr-Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S., & Leary, D. E. (Eds.). (1992). A century of psychology as science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1938). The place of value in a world of facts. Oxford, UK: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: How to follow scientists and engineers through society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993a). The pasteurisation of France. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993b). We have never been modern. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the social: An introduction to actor-network theory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts. Princeton, NJ.: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Law, J., & Mol, A. (1995). Notes on materiality and sociality. The Sociological Review,43(2), 274–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leahey, T. H. (2017). A history of psychology: From antiquity to modernity. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Leonelli, S. (2016a). Data centric biology: A philosophical study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Leonelli, S. (2016b). Locating ethics in data science: Responsibility and accountability in global and distributed knowledge production systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences,374(2083), 20160122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombrozo, T., Knobe, J., & Nichols, S. (Eds.). (2014). Oxford studies in experimental philosophy (Vol. 1). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mach, E. (1896). Contributions to the analysis of the sensations (C. M. Williams, Trans.). Chicago: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, K., & Barbarino, G. (2009). Narrations of race in STEM settings: Identity formation and its discontents. Science Education,93(3), 48–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. (1966). The psychology of science: A reconnaissance. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (1843). A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive: Being a connected view of the principles of evidence and the methods of scientific investigation (Vol. 1). London: John W. Parker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitroff, I. (1974). The subjective side of science: Philosophical inquiry into the psychology of the Apollo moon scientists. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morawski, J. G. (Ed.). (1988). The rise of experimentation in American psychology. New Haven, CT, US: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1984). Faraday to Einstein: Constructing meaning in scientific theories. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff/Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (1992). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In R. Giere (Ed.), Cognitive models of science (pp. 3–44). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Doherty, K., & Einsiedel, E. (Eds.). (2013). Public engagement and emerging technologies. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osbeck, L., & Nersessian, N. (2015). Prolegomena to an empirical philosophy of science. In S. Wagenknecht, N. J. Nersessian, & H. Andersen (Eds.), Empirical philosophy of science: Introducing qualitative methods into philosophy of science (pp. 13–35). Cham: Springer International.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, D. (2003). The ordinary superstition of subjectivity: Liberalism and technostructural violence. Theory & Psychology,13(1), 73–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, I. (2014). Managing neo-liberalism and the strong state in higher education: Psychology today. Qualitative Research in Psychology,11(3), 250–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1878). How to make our ideas clear. Popular Science Monthly, 12(January), 286–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickren, W., & Rutherford, A. (2010). A history of modern psychology in context. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1964). Science, faith, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (Originally published 1946).

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1974). Personal knowledge: Towards a post-critical philosophy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (Originally published 1958).

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (2002). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Routledge (Originally published 1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (Eds.). (2012). Psychology of science: Implicit and explicit processes. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichenbach, H. (1938). Experience and prediction: An analysis of the foundations and the structure of knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. N. (1995). An intellectual history of psychology. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roe, A. (1953). A psychological study of eminent psychologists and anthropologists, and a comparison with biological and physical scientists. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied,67(2), 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., & Carrier, L. M. (2015). The Wiley handbook of psychology, technology, and society. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schachtner, C. (2013). Digital media evoking interactive games in virtual space. Subjectivity,6(1), 33–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaffer, S. (1988). Astronomers mark time: Discipline and the personal equation. Science in Context,2(1), 115–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraube, E. (2009). Technology as materialized action and its ambivalences. Theory & Psychology,19(2), 296–312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354309103543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraube, E. (2013). First-person perspective and sociomaterial decentering: Studying technology from the standpoint of the subject. Subjectivity,6(1), 12–32. https://doi.org/10.1057/sub.2012.28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraube, E., & Højholt, C. (Eds.). (2016). Psychology and the conduct of everyday life. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraube, E., & Marvakis, A. (2016). Frozen fluidity: Digital technologies and the transformation of students learning and conduct of everyday life. In E. Schraube & C. Højholt (Eds.), Psychology and the conduct of everyday life (pp. 205–225). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1966). The psychology of scientific problem solving. In R. Tweney, M. Doherty, & C. Mynatt (Eds.), On scientific thinking (pp. 48–54). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1988). Scientific genius: A psychology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, B. (2004). Technology and the African-American experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Alfred Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slaney, K. (2017). Validating psychological constructs: Historical, philosophical, and practical dimensions. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Søndergaard, D. M. (2013). Virtual materiality, potentiality and subjectivity: How do we conceptualize real-virtual interaction embodied and enacted in computer gaming, imagery and night dreams? Subjectivity,6(1), 55–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sørensen, E. (2009). The materiality of learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sørensen, E., & Schraube, E. (Eds.). (2013). Special issue: “Materiality”. Subjectivity, 6(1), 1–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. W., & Barron, F. (Eds.). (1963). Scientific creativity: Its recognition and development. Oxford, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teo, T. (2015). Critical psychology: A geography of intellectual engagement and resistance. American Psychologist,70(3), 243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teo, T. (2017). From psychological science to the psychological humanities: Building a general theory of subjectivity. Review of General Psychology,21(4), 281–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tissaw, M. A. (2007). Making sense of neonatal imitation. Theory & Psychology,17(2), 217–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkle, S. (1984). The second self: Computers and the human spirit. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle, S. (Ed.). (2008). The inner history of devices. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle, S. (2015). Reclaiming conversation: The power of talk in a digital age. New York: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tweney, R. D. (1989). A framework for the cognitive psychology of science. In B. Gholson, W. R. Shadish Jr., R. A. Neimeyer, & A. C. Houts (Eds.), Psychology of science: Contributions to metascience (pp. 342–366). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tweney, R. D., Doherty, M. E., & Mynatt, C. R. (Eds.). (1981). On scientific thinking. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2014). An invitation to cultural psychology. London: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. (1995). The recent progress of the theory of vision. In D. Cahan (Ed.), Science and culture: Popular and philosophical essays (pp. 127–203). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press (Originally published 1868).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotzky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagenknecht, S., Nersessian, N. J., & Andersen, H. (Eds.). (2015). Empirical philosophy of science: Introducing qualitative methods into philosophy of science (vol. 2). Springer. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-18600-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. L. (1938). Scientists are human. London: Watts & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1981). Einstein: The thinking that led to the theory of relativity. In R. Tweney, M. Doherty, & C. Mynatt (Eds.), On scientific thinking (pp. 192–211). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whewell, W. (1847). The philosophy of the inductive sciences (2 Vols.). London: John W. Parker.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winner, L. (1989). The whale and the reactor: A search for limits in an age of high technology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations (G. E. M. Anscombe & R. Rhees, Eds., G. E. M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kieran C. O’Doherty .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

O’Doherty, K.C., Osbeck, L.M., Schraube, E., Yen, J. (2019). Introduction: Psychological Studies of Science and Technology. In: O’Doherty, K.C., Osbeck, L.M., Schraube, E., Yen, J. (eds) Psychological Studies of Science and Technology. Palgrave Studies in the Theory and History of Psychology. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-25308-0_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics