Advertisement

Examining the Influence of Visual Stimuli and Personal Characteristics on Users’ Willingness-to-Wait Time and Waiting Patterns

  • Jingyi Zhou
  • Pengyi ZhangEmail author
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 11584)

Abstract

Waiting is inevitable in many interaction scenarios. Prior research suggests that waiting experience is associated with factors at both individual and contextual levels. This paper reports an experiment study examining the influence of visual stimuli and personal characteristics on users’ waiting behavior. Our results show that higher delayed gratification (individual) is associated with longer waiting duration and shorter strategy formation time. As for features of visual stimuli (contextual), rotating-bright loading icons lengthen time perception and discourage people from waiting. These results provide design implications such as personalized waiting duration and ways to create better waiting experience.

Keywords

Waiting Delayed gratification Loading icon Time perception 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research is supported by the “Provost’s Undergraduate Research Fellowship” of Peking University and by NSFC Grant #71603012.

References

  1. 1.
    Rung, J.M., Young, M.E.: Learning to wait for more likely or just more: greater tolerance to delays of reward with increasingly longer delays. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 103(1), 108–124 (2015)Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Ghafurian, M., Reitter, D.: Impatience induced by waiting: an effect moderated by the speed of countdowns. In: Proceedings of the 2016 ACM Conference on Designing Interactive Systems. ACM (2016)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Söderström, U., Bååth, M., Mejtoft, T.: The Users’ Time Perception: the effect of various animation speeds on loading screens. In: Proceedings of the 36th European Conference on Cognitive Ergonomics. ACM (2018)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Hefer, B., Karabenick, S.A.: Inherent association between academic delay of gratification, future time perspective, and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 16(1), 35–57 (2004)Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Nah, F.-H.F.: A study on tolerable waiting time: how long are web users willing to wait? Behav. Inf. Technol. 23(3), 153–163 (2004)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Nielson Norman Group website page. https://www.nngroup.com/articles/website-response-times/. Accessed 29 Jan 2018
  7. 7.
    Galletta, D.F., Henry, R., McCoy, S., Polak, P.: Web site delays: how tolerant are users? J. Assoc. Inf. Syst. 5(1), 1–28 (2004)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Ceaparu, I., Lazar, J., Bessiere, K., Robinson, J., Shneiderman, B.: Determining causes and severity of end-user frustration. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Interact. 17(3), 333–356 (2004)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Hohenstein, J., Khan, H., Canfield, K., Tung, S., Cano, R.P.: Shorter wait times: the effects of various loading screens on perceived performance. In: Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM (2016)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Nielson Norman Group website page. https://www.nngroup.com/articles/progress-indicators/. Accessed 29 Jan 2018
  11. 11.
    Kim, W., Xiong, S., Liang, Z.: Effect of loading symbol of online video on perception of waiting time. Int. J. Hum.-Comput. Interact. 33(12), 1001–1009 (2017)Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Mcguire, J.T., Kable, J.W.: Decision makers calibrate behavioral persistence on the basis of time-interval experience. Cognition 124(2), 216–226 (2012)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Mischel, W., Cantor, N., Feldman, S.: Principles of self-regulation: the nature of willpower and self-control. In: Higgins, E.T., Kruglanski, A.W. (eds.) Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, pp. 329–360. Guilford Press, New York (1996)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Mcguire, J.T., Kable, J.W.: Rational temporal predictions can underlie apparent failures to delay gratification. Psychol. Rev. 120(2), 395–410 (2013)Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E.B.: Attention in delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 16(2), 329–337 (1970)Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., Rodriguez, M.: Delay of gratification in children. Science 244(4907), 933–938 (1989)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Wulfert, E., Block, J.A., Santa, A.E., Rodriguez, M.L., Colsman, M.: Delay of gratification: impulsive choices and problem behaviors in early and late adolescence. J. Pers. 70(4), 533–552 (2010)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Casey, B.J., et al.: Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108(36), 14998–15003 (2011)Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Toner, I.J., Smith, R.A.: Age and overt verbalization in delay-maintenance behavior in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 24(1), 123–128 (1977)Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Matthews, W.J., Meck, W.H.: Temporal cognition: connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychol. Bull. 142(8), 865–907 (2016)Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Lhamon, W.T., Goldstone, S.: Movement and the judged duration of visual targets. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 5(1), 53–54 (1975)Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Brown, S.W.: Time, change, and motion: the effects of stimulus movement on temporal perception. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 57(1), 105–116 (1995)Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Kanai, R., Paffen, C.L., Hogendoorn, H., Verstraten, F.A.: Time dilation in dynamic visual display. J. Vis. 6(12), 8 (2006)Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Goldstone, S., Lhamon, W.T., Sechzer, J.A.: Light intensity and judged duration. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 12(1), 83–84 (1978)Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Hoerger, M., Quirk, S.W., Weed, N.C.: Development and validation of the Delaying Gratification Inventory. Psychol. Assess. 23(3), 725–738 (2011)Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Zur, H.B., Breznitz, S.: The effect of time pressure on risky choice behavior. Acta Physiol. (Oxf) 47(2), 89–104 (1981)Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Edland, A., Svenson, O.: Judgment and decision making under time pressure: studies and findings. In: Svenson, O., Maule, A.J. (eds.) Time Pressure and Stress in Human Judgment and Decision Making, pp. 27–40. Springer, Boston (1993).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-6846-6_2Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Bruno, A., Johnston, A.: Contrast gain shapes visual time. Front. Psychol. 1(6), 170 (2010)Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Bouch, A., Kuchinsky, A., Bhatti, N.: Quality is in the eye of the beholder: meeting users’ requirements for Internet quality of service. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM (2000)Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Jang, J., Suk, H.: Disappearing icons: informative effect through changing color attributes of app icons. In: 2014 IEEE International Conference on Consumer Electronics (ICCE). IEEE (2014)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Psychological and Cognitive SciencesPeking UniversityBeijingChina
  2. 2.Department of Information ManagementPeking UniversityBeijingChina

Personalised recommendations