Skip to main content

From Belief to Behaviour

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychology of Micro-Targeted Election Campaigns

Abstract

Our beliefs about the world are integral to how we can act within it. It may be tempting to assume that voters will support the candidate they believe is the best (or least bad) person for the job. Yet, beliefs do not deterministically yield corresponding behaviours. Competing preferences, inability, or force of habit may lead to behaviours that are incommensurate with our belief or intentions (failure to exercise regularly is a testament to this). Due to this, campaigns must employ strategies to change people’s beliefs and to influence their behaviour. This chapter explores the oft-fraught relationship between beliefs and behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The rule is: (VijDj)2 – (VijRj)2 where Vi represents the voter’s ideal position and Dj and Rj represent the ideological positions of the candidate in area j.

  2. 2.

    This depresses small parties and typically results in parliaments with 2–3 main parties compared with proportional representation, which often has parliaments with 6–9 parties.

  3. 3.

    It is unclear whether this is causally connected or correlated, as people who are more likely to vote might also be more likely to be friends, as they share political interests and may feel similarly about civic responsibility. Regardless, the impact of social networks and emulation of friends and family is palpable.

  4. 4.

    If this model of the voter is incorrect, it may have disastrous effects. We discuss the pitfalls of poor model assumptions, mistaken data analyses, or bad testing in Chap. 8.

  5. 5.

    There are numerous emotional states, which complicates the research (as each type of emotion may influence reasoning and decision-making differently). Foundationally, Ekman and Friesen (1971) argue for six basic emotions: anger, fear, disgust, surprise, happiness, and sadness.

  6. 6.

    For a general model of voters, see Lodge and Taber (2013).

Bibliography

  • Ajzen, I. (1980). From Intentions to Action: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action-Control: From Cognition to Behavior (pp. 11–39). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of Goal-Directed Behavior: Attitudes, Intentions and Perceived Behavioral Control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 453–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. (2011). Talking to the Enemy: Violent Extremism, Sacred Values, and What It Means to Be Human. Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (2009). Cognitive Biases in Moral Judgments that Affect Political Behavior. Synthese, 172(1), 7–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, P. A., Dalton, R. T., Greene, S., & Huckfeldt, R. (2002). The Social Calculus of Voting: Interpersonal, Media, and Organizational Influences on Presidential Choices. The American Political Science Review, 96(1), 57–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beier, K., & Buchstein, H. (2004). George E Marcus: The Sentimental Citizen. Emotion in Democratic Politics. Politische Viertaljahresschrift, 45(2), 284–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Campbell, M. (2005). The Effect of Emotion on Syllogistic Reasoning in a Group of War Veterans. Paper presented at the XXVIIth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Stresa, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The Influence of Affect on Higher Level Cognition: A Review of Research on Interpretation, Judgement, Decision Making and Reasoning. Cognition & Emotion, 24(4), 561–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury, A., McGimpsey, I., & Santori, D. (2013). Revising Rationality. Journal of Education Policy, 28(2), 247–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braha, D., & de Aguiar, M. A. (2017). Voter Contagion: Modelling and Analysis of a Century of U.S. Presidential Elections. PLoS One, 12(5), e0177970.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Channon, S., & Baker, J. (1994). Reasoning Strategies in Depression: Effects of Depressed Mood on a Syllogism Task. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 707–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N. (2018). The Mind Is Flat: The Illusion of Mental Depth and the Improvised Mind. Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. (2007). Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. Collins Business.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M., & Abraham, C. (2001). Conscientiousness and the Theory of Planned Behavior: Toward a More Complete Model of the Antecedents of Intentions and Behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1547–1561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Constans, J. I. (2001). Worry Propensity and the Perception of Risk. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 721–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constans, J. I., & Mathews, A. M. (1993). Mood and the Subjective Risk of Future Events. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 545–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courneya, K. S., Bobick, T. M., & Schinke, R. J. (1999). Does the Theory of Planned Behavior Mediate the Relation Between Personality and Exercise Behavior? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(4), 317–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (2006). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. Putnam Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delli, C. X. M., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans Know About Politics and Why It Matters. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1998). Working-memory Capacity in High Trait-Anxious and Repressor Groups. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 697–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, K. A. (2014). What Does Gender Matter? Women Candidates and Gender Stereotypes in American Elections. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, B. (2018). The Perils of Perception: Why We Are Wrong About Nearly Everything. Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants Across Cultures in the Face and Emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17(2), 124–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Sorensen, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-Cultural Elements in Facial Displays of Emotion. Science. New Series, 164(3875), 86–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, J. (2013). Agent_Zero: Toward Neurocognitive Foundations for Generative Social Science. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Mogg, K., May, J., Richards, A., & Mathews, A. (1991). Bias in Interpretation of Ambiguous Sentences Related to Threat in Anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 144–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M. (1980). A Theory of Reasoned Action: Some Applications and Implications. In H. Howe & M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 65–116). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, B., Bruijn, G. J., & Lally, P. (2011). A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Applications of the Self-Report Habit Index to Nutrition and Physical Activity Behaviours. Annals of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 42(2), 174–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilens, M. (2001). Political Ignorance and Collective Policy Preferences. The American Political Science Review, 95(2), 379–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, B. T., Hansford, T. G., & Krause, G. A. (2007). The Republicans Should Pray for Rain: Weather, Turnout, and Voting in U.S. Presidential Elections. The Journal of Politics, 69(3), 649–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. P., & Gerber, A. S. (2008). Get Out the Vote: How to Increase Voter Turnout. Brookings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G. (2002). It’s Easy to Like or Dislike Tony Blair: Accessibility Experiences and the Favourability of Attitude Judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 257–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, L., Johansson, P., & Strandberg, T. (2012). Lifting the Veil of Morality: Choice Blindness and Attitude-Reversals on a Self-transformation Survey. PLoS One, 7(9), e45457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, L., Johansson, P., Tärning, B., Sikström, S., & Deutgen, T. (2010). Magic at the Marketplace: Choice Blindness for the Taste of Jam and the Smell of Tea. Cognition, 117(1), 54–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, L., Strandberg, T., Pärnamets, P., Lind, A., Tärning, D., & Johansson, P. (2013). How the Polls Can Be Spot on and Dead Wrong: Using Choice Blindness to Shift Political Attitudes and Voter Intentions. PLoS One, 8(4), e60554.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, A. J., Malhotra, N., & Mo, C. H. (2010). Irrelevant Events Affect Voters’ Evaluations of Government Performance. PNAS, 107(29), 12804–12809.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hood, B. (2012). The Self Illusion: Why There Is No ‘You’ Inside Your Head. London: Constable & Robinson Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, G. A., Hill, S. J., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Sources of Bias in Retrospective Decision-making: Experimental Evidence on Voters’ Limitations in Controlling Incumbents. American Political Science Review, 106(4), 720–741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1985). A Treatise of Human Nature. Penguin Classics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust Sensitivity, Political Conservatism, and Voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jessee, S. A. (2010). Partisan Bias, Political Information and Spatial Voting in the 2008 Presidential Election. Journal of Politics, 72(2), 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jessee, S. A. (2012). Ideology and Spatial Voting in American Elections. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joesten, D. A., & Stone, W. J. (2014). Reassessing Proximity Voting: Expertise, Party, and Choice in Congressional Elections. The Journal of Politics, 76(3), 740–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, P., Hall, L., Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to Detect Mismatches Between Intention and Outcome in a Simple Decision Task. Science, 310, 116–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinder, D. R. (1998). Opinion and Action in the Realm of Politics. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 778–867). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosters, M., & Heijden, J. v. d. (2015). From Mechanism to Virtue: Evaluating Nudge Theory. Evaluation, 21(3), 276–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyogoku, J., & Ike, N. (1960). Urban-rural Differences in Voting Behavior in Postwar Japan. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 9(1), 167–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., Patel, P., Fahmy, D. F., & Kaufman, R. R. (2013). Correct Voting Across Thirty-Three Democracies: A Preliminary Analysis. British Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 239–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, R. R., & Redlawsk, D. P. (1997). Voting Correctly. American Political Science Review, 91(3), 585–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laustsen, L. (2017). Choosing the Right Candidate: Observational and Experimental Evidence that Conservatives and Liberals Prefer Powerful and Warm Candidate Personalities, Respectively. Political Behavior, 39, 883–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lefford, A. (1946). The Influence of Emotional Subject Matter on Logical Reasoning. The Journal of General Psychology, 34(2), 127–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, A. (2009). Does the World Economy Swing National Elections? Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 71(2), 163–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, G., & Razin, R. (2015). Correlation Neglect, Voting Behavior, and Information Aggregation. The American Economic Review, 105(4), 1634–1645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M., & Taber, C. S. (2013). The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martino, B. d., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, Biases, and Rational Decision-Making in the Human Brain. Science, 313, 684–687.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent Judgment Is a General Effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michie, S., Atkins, L., & West, R. (2014). The Behaviour Change Wheel: A Guide to Designing Interventions. Silverback Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T. (1999). The Norm of Self-Interest. American Psychologist, 54(12), 1053–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. R. (2011). The Emotional Citizen: Emotion as a Function of Political Sophistication. Political Psychology, 32(4), 575–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B., & Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Mood, Reasoning, and Central Executive Processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 476–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panagopoulos, C. (2010). Affect, Social Pressure and Prosocial Motivation: Field Experimental Evidence of the Mobilizing Effects of Pride, Shame, and Publicizing Voting Behavior. Political Behavior, 32(3), 369–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, M. T., & Isbell, L. M. (2010). How I Vote Depends on How I Feel: The Differential Impact of Anger and Fear on Political Information Processing. Psychological Science, 21(4), 548–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rezlescu, C., Duchaine, B., Olivola, C. Y., & Chater, N. (2012). Unfakeable Facial Configuration Affect Strategic Choice in Trust Games With or Without Information About Past Behaviour. PLoS One, 7(3), e34293.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, R. E., & Courneya, K. S. (2003). Relationships Between Personality, an Extended Theory of Planned Behaviour Model and Exercise Behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8(1), 19–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosling, H. (2018). Factfulness: Ten Reasons We’re Wrong About the World – And Why Things Are Better Than You Think. Sceptre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Universal Vocal Signals of Emotion. In N. Taatgen & H. v. Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2251–2255).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Cross-cultural Recognition of Basic Emotions Through Nonverbal Emotional Vocalization. PNAS, 107(6), 2408–2412.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G. (2001). Emotion Inferences from Vocal Expression Correlate Across Languages and Cultures. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 32, 76–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shor, B., & Rogowski, J. C. (2012). Congressional Voting by Spatial Reasoning, 2000–2010. Working Paper. University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R., & Sunstein, C. (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toderov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A., & Hall, C. C. (2005). Inferences of Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes. Science, 308, 1623–1626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. C. (1980). Values, Attitudes, and Interpersonal Behavior. In H. Howe & M. Page (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium of Motivation (Vol. 27, pp. 195–259). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., Banks, A. J., & Davis, A. K. (2008). Is a Worried Citizen a Good Citizen? Emotions, Political Information Seeking, and Learning via the Internet. Political Psychology, 29(2), 247–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Knutson, B., Paulus, M., & Trujillo, J. L. (2007). Affective Influence on Judgments and Decisions: Moving Towards Core Mechanisms. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Non-academic References

  • FullFact. (2018, February 15). UK Spending on Foreign Aid.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (2016, November 9). Most People Clueless on U.S. Foreign Aid Spending. PolitiFact.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jens Koed Madsen .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Madsen, J.K. (2019). From Belief to Behaviour. In: The Psychology of Micro-Targeted Election Campaigns. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-22145-4_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics