Skip to main content

The Theoretical and Methodological Framework. Semiotic Cultural Psychology, Symbolic Universes and Lines of Semiotic Forces

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Symbolic Universes in Time of (Post)Crisis

Abstract

The analyses and discussions developed throughout the volume are based on the Semiotic Cultural Psychological Theory (SCPT). SCPT integrates relational psychoanalysis, Dynamic Systems Theory and pragmatic semiotics within the more general framework of socio-cultural psychology. SCPT conceives mental processes as ongoing dynamics of sensemaking. Sensemaking consists of processes of interpretation of the world that shape experience. These interpretation processes are guided by generalized, affect-laden meanings that reflect the cultural milieu and that work as basic intuitive assumptions about the world—what is and how it works—as a whole. SCPT adopts the term “symbolic universes” to denote such systems of assumptions. A symbolic universe is an affect-laden, pre-semantic meaning working as a basic, generalized assumption that shapes the experience of both the outer (i.e. the social and physical space) and inner environment (i.e., the experience of one’s body and feelings), namely, the embodied image sensemakers have of themselves and of their relation with the world. In their turn, a symbolic universe can be interpreted as the emergent effect of the interplay of a certain set of polarized, generalized embodied latent dimension of sense—defined “lines of semiotic force” by SCPT. This chapter outlines how symbolic universes and lines of semiotic force can be detected and the role they play in social development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ahuja, A., Kremer, M., & Zwane, A. P. (2010). Providing safe water: Evidence from randomized evaluations. Annual Review of Resource Economics, 2(1), 237–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. (2001). The emergence of meaning: Generating symbols from random sounds—A factor analytic model. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 8(2), 101–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arrow, K. J., & Debreu, G. (1954). Existence of an equilibrium for a competitive economy. Econometrica, 22, 265–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickard, M. H. (2009). Interactionism: A manifesto. New Ideas in Psychology, 27(1), 85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. (original work in French, 1972).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 42, 33–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1947). Emotional selectivity in perception and reaction. Journal of Personality, 16, 69–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1949). On the perception of incongruity: A paradigm. Journal of Personality, 8, 206–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciavolino, E., Redd, R., Avdi, E., Falcone, F., Fini, V., Kadianaki, I., et al. (2017). Views of context. An instrument for the analysis of the cultural milieu. A first validation study. Electronic Journal of Applied Statistical Analysis, 10(2), 599–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobern, W. W., & Aikenhead, G. (1997). Cultural aspects of learning science. Scientific Literacy and Cultural Studies Project, (13). http://scholarworks.wmich.edu/science_slcsp/13.

  • Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activity approach. American Sociological Review, 44(4), 588–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology. A once and future discipline. Cambridge Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, Mass: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure, F. (2011). Course in general linguistics. Losanna, Parigi: Payot. (original work in French, 1916).

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, M. (1986). How institutions think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupas, P., & Robinson, J. (2013). Why don’t the poor save more? Evidence from health savings experiments. American Economic Review, 103(4), 1138–1171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • European Commission. (1998). Communication from the Commission on undeclared work. COM (98) 219 final, 7 April 1998 [EU Commission—COM Document].

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1983). Local knowledge: Further essays in interpretive anthropology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2008). Why heuristics work. Perspective in Psychological Science, 3(1), 20–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. (Eds.). (1999). Simple heuristic that make us smart. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., Grif, D., & Kahneman, D. (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). A fine is a price. Journal of Legal Studies, 29(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109, 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R., & Gillett, G. (1994). The discursive mind. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. (2011). Cultural psychology. San Francisco: W. W. Norton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H. J. M. (2001). The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology, 7(3), 243–281. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X0173001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ISBEM. (2017). TR Synchronic Analysis [Scientific report. Re.Cri.Re Deliverable 3.2]. http://www.recrire.eu/documents.

  • Kahan, D. M. (2015). Climate-science communication and the measurement problem. Advances in Political Psychology, 36, 1–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449–1475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (2000). Choices, values, and frames. New York/Cambridge: Russell Sage Foundation/Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, M. (1967). Contribution to psychoanalysis, 1921–1945. New York: Mac Graw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laura-Grotto, R. P., Salvatore, S., Gennaro, A., & Gelo, O. (2009). The unbearable dynamicity of psychological processes: Highlights of the psychodynamic theories. In J. Valsiner, P. Molenaar, M. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamics process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 1–30). New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lévy Strauss, C. (1958). Antropologie structurale [Structural Anthropology]. Paris: Plon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1947). Frontiers in group dynamics: Concept, method and reality in social science; social equilibria and social change. Human Relations, 1, 5–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872674700100103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking language, mind and world dialogically. Interactional and contextual theories of sense-making. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, G. (1996). Ending footbinding and infibulation: A convention account. American Sociological Review, 61(6), 999–1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mannarini, T. M., Velti, G. M., & Salvatore, S. (in preparation). Portrays of otherness. Mapping change and continuity in the European media representations of immigrants, Muslims, and LGBT. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manzotti, R. (2010). There are no Images (to be seen) or the fallacy of the intermediate entity. APA Newsletter on Philosophy and Computers, 9, 59–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markova, I. (2003). Dialogicality and social representations. The dynamics of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matte Blanco, I. (1975). The unconsious as infinite sets. An essays in bi-logic. London: Gerald Duckworth & Company Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S. A. (1988). Relational concepts in psychoanalysis. An integration. Cambridge Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. (1961). La psychanalyse, son image, son public [Psychoanalysis, its image, its public]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mossi, P. G., & Salvatore, S. (2011). Psychological transition from meaning to sense. European Journal of Education and Psychology, 4(2), 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1932). In C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss (Eds.), Collected papers of Charles Sanders Peirce (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (original version, 1897).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, C. (2008). Cultural psychology and qualitative methodology: Scientific and political considerations. Culture & Psychology, 14(3), 259–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative. Towards a theory of language and mind (pp. 19–44). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosa, A., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2018). Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S. (2012). Social life of the sign: Sensemaking in society. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 241–254). Oxford: Oxford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S. (2013). The reciprocal inherency of self and context. Outline for a semiotic model of constitution of experience. Interacçioes, 9(24), 20–50. http://www.eses.pt/interaccoes.

  • Salvatore, S. (2016). Psychology in black and white. The project of a theory-driven science. Charlotte, NC: InfoAge Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Fini, V., Mannarini, T., Veltri, G. A., Avdi, E., Battaglia, F., et al. (2018a). Symbolic universes between present and future of Europe. First results of the map of European societies’ cultural milieu. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0189885. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Freda, M. F. (2011). Affect, unconscious and sensemaking. A psychodynamic, semiotic and dialogic model. New Ideas in Psychology, 29, 119–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Lauro-Grotto, R., Gennaro, A., & Gelo, O. (2009). Attempts to grasp the dynamicity of intersubjectivity. In J. Valsiner, P. Molenaar, M. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamics process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 171–190). New York, NY: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Mannarini, T., Avdi, E., Battaglia F., Cremaschi, M., Forges Davanzati, et al. (2018b, on line first). Globalization, demand of sense and enemization of the other. A psycho-cultural analysis of European societies’ socio-political crisis. Culture & Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Tonti, M., & Gennaro, A. (2017). How to model sensemaking. A contribution for the development of a methodological framework for the analysis of meaning. In M. Han & C. Cunha (Eds.), The subjectified and subjectifying mind (pp. 245–268). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Tschacher, W. (2012). Time dependency of psychotherapeutic exchanges: The contribution of the theory of dynamic systems in analyzing process. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 253. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory & Psychology, 20(6), 817–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Venuleo, C. (2013). Field dependency and contingency in the modelling of sensemaking. Papers on Social Representation [On Line Journal], 22(2), 21.1–21.41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Zittoun, T. (2011). Outlines of a psychoanalytically informed cultural psychology. In S. Salvatore & T. Zittoun (Eds.), Cultural psychology and psychoanalysis in dialogue. Issues for constructive theoretical and methodological synergies (pp. 3–46). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1967). The phenomenology of the social world. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A. (2000). The psychology of practice and the practice of the three psychologies. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3(3), 207–222. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.00065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, R. (1991). Psychoanalytic theories of affect. London: Karnac Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storolow, R., Brandchaft, B., & Atwood, G. E. (1995). Psychoanalytic treatment: An intersubjective approach. New York: The Analytic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tahler, R. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4(3), 199–214. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.4.3.199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tonti, M., & Salvatore, S. (2015). The homogenization of classification functions measurement (HOCFUN): A method for measuring the salience of emotional arousal in thinking. American Journal of Psychology, 128(4), 469–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ugazio, V. (2013). Semantic polarities in the family. Permitted and forbidden stories. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind. Cambridge Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in minds and societies. Foundations of cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2014). An invitation to cultural psychology. London: Sage Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., & Rosa, A. (Eds.). (2007). The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Visetti, Y. M., & Cadiot, P. (2002). Instability and theory of semantic forms. Starting from the case of prepositions. In S. Feigenbaum & D. Kurzon (Eds.), Prepositions in their syntactic, semantic and pragmatic context (pp. 9–39). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waltz, K. N. (2010). Theory of international politics. Long Grove, ILL: Waveland Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • WHO (World Health Organization). (1999). Female genital mutilation programmes to date: What works and what doesn’t. Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. Basil Blackwell: Oxford. (original publication in German, 1953).

    Google Scholar 

  • World Bank. (2015). World development report 2015: Mind, society, and behavior. Washington, DC: World Bank.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sergio Salvatore .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Salvatore, S., Valsiner, J., Veltri, G.A. (2019). The Theoretical and Methodological Framework. Semiotic Cultural Psychology, Symbolic Universes and Lines of Semiotic Forces. In: Salvatore, S., Fini, V., Mannarini, T., Valsiner, J., Veltri, G. (eds) Symbolic Universes in Time of (Post)Crisis. Culture in Policy Making: The Symbolic Universes of Social Action. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-19497-0_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-19497-0_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-19496-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-19497-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics