Skip to main content

Born to Be Mild? Cohort Effects Don’t (Fully) Explain Why Well-Being Is U-Shaped in Age

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover The Economics of Happiness

Abstract

The statistical analysis of cross-section data very often reveals a U-shaped relationship between subjective well-being and age. This paper uses 18 waves of British panel data to try to distinguish between two potential explanations of this shape: a pure life-cycle or aging effect, and a fixed cohort effect depending on year of birth. Panel analysis controlling for fixed effects continues to produce a U-shaped relationship between well-being and age, although this U-shape is flatter for life satisfaction than for the GHQ measure of mental well-being. The pattern of the estimated cohort effects also differs between the two well-being measures and, to an extent, by demographic group. In particular, those born earlier report more positive GHQ scores, controlling for their current age; this phenomenon is especially prevalent for women.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The sample in Mroczek and Spiro (2005) actually consists of veterans over the age of 40, so that their finding of an upward-sloping profile is not inconsistent with a U-shape over all ages.

  2. 2.

    Graham and Ruiz Pozuelo (2017) find a U-shape in 44 out of 46 countries investigated in Gallup World Poll data. This conclusion does not however hold for all well-being measures. In Stone et al. (2010), a U-shape is found for the Cantril ladder and positive emotions in Gallup data, but not for negative emotions.

  3. 3.

    Putnam (2000), page 141, concludes that the decline in trust in the US is purely a cohort phenomenon, with each cohort’s trust not changing over time.

  4. 4.

    This is the conclusion reached by Easterlin and Schaeffer (1999), using 20 years of cohort data from the US General Social Survey. Kassenböhmer and Haisken-DeNew (2012) suggest that the U-shape in German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) data is entirely explained by individual fixed effects and experience in the panel. Cribier (2005) is an evocative account of the differences in life experience between two cohorts of French workers born only 14 years apart.

  5. 5.

    The wave 1 sample was drawn from 250 areas of Great Britain. Additional samples of 1500 households in each of Scotland and Wales were added to the main sample in 1999, and in 2001 a sample of 2000 households was added in Northern Ireland.

  6. 6.

    More precisely: whether subjective well-being changes systematically in a way that cannot be explained by the standard set of explanatory variables (covering income, employment, health, demographics etc.).

  7. 7.

    One worry is that the GHQ is singularly unsuitable for this kind of analysis, as its constituent parts are explicitly phrased in terms of comparisons to usual. It is worth noting that the empirical literature on GHQ scores treats them unambiguously as indicators of the level of well-being, and it was for this purpose that the instrument was designed. On a practical level, the employed’s GHQ is more strongly correlated with job satisfaction levels in the BHPS data than with job satisfaction changes. Last, with 18 years of balanced panel data, a relatively direct test of the usefulness of the GHQ score in this respect can be envisaged. If events become more ‘usual’ as an individual ages, then the standard deviation of GHQ scores (and of its individual components) will fall with age. There is no evidence of this phenomenon in balanced BHPS panel data.

  8. 8.

    Alternatively, the responses to the GHQ-12 questions can be used to construct what is known as a Likert measure. This is the simple sum of the responses to the 12 questions, coded so that the response with the lowest well-being value scores 3 and that with the highest well-being value scores 0. This count is then reversed, so that higher scores indicate higher levels of well-being. The measure thus runs from 0 (all 12 responses indicating the worst psychological health) to 36 (all responses indicating the best psychological health). Practically, the results are very similar between the Caseness and Likert measures.

  9. 9.

    Income is measured in real terms, having been deflated by the CPI.

  10. 10.

    Nolen-Hoeksema and Rusting (1999) conclude in their survey article that women exhibit higher incidence rates for almost all of the mood and anxiety disorders, but in general report higher levels of happiness.

  11. 11.

    Blanchflower and Oswald (2004) explicitly do not control for health in their statistical analysis.

  12. 12.

    Alternatively, interaction terms between age and wave can be introduced into Table 1’s regressions; these give qualitatively very similar results.

  13. 13.

    Blanchflower and Oswald (2004) carry out a similar test on American General Social Survey data, and conclude that there is only slight evidence that the minimum moves to the right over time. The GSS is not, however, a panel.

  14. 14.

    This is also the approach is taken in Blanchflower and Oswald (2008), where the cohort appears in 10-year blocks. Wunder et al. (2013) do not introduce cohort effects at all, and estimate the age profile semi-parametrically in BHPS and SOEP data, which produces a U-shape (deeper in the BHPS) up until the age of retirement.

  15. 15.

    It has often been observed that income is hump-shaped in age, for example, so that holding income constant will deepen the well-being U-shape. Equally, Glaeser et al. (2002) suggest that social capital is hump-shaped in age. Frijters and Beatton (2012) find that introducing controls deepens the U-shape in their analysis of SOEP, Australian HILDA and BHPS panel data.

  16. 16.

    Easterlin (2006) reaches a similar conclusion for happiness in US cohorts.

  17. 17.

    Blanchflower and Oswald (2008) also find a U-shaped pattern of birth cohort effects in life satisfaction, using 27 years of Eurobarometer data.

  18. 18.

    Such comparisons to the past imply that, in the long run, the correlation between GDP per capita and individual well-being may well be small. For some recent empirical contributions to this debate, see Diener and Oishi (2000), Easterlin (1995) and Oswald (1997). This literature is surveyed in Clark et al. (2008).

References

  • Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2). New York/London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (1989). The psychology of happiness. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Well-being over time in Britain and the USA. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1359–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2008). Is wellbeing U-shaped over the life cycle? Social Science and Medicine, 66, 1733–1749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of twins reared apart. Science, 250, 223–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, T., Oswald, A. J., & Powdthavee, N. (2017). Longitudinal evidence for a midlife nadir in human well-being: Results from four data sets. Economic Journal, 127, 126–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. (2008). Relative income, happiness and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin Paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cribier, F. (2005). Changes in the experience of life between two cohorts of Parisian pensioners, born in circa 1907 and 1921. Aging & Society, 25, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Money and happiness: Income and subjective well-being across nations. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Cross-cultural psychology of subjective well-being. Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (2006). Life cycle happiness and its sources. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27, 463–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A., & Schaeffer, C. M. (1999). Income and subjective well-being over the life-cycle. In C. D. Ryff & V. W. Marshall (Eds.), The self and society in aging processes. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? Economic Journal, 114, 641–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1989). Frames of reference and the quality of life. American Economic Review, 79, 80–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S. & Stutzer, A. (2002). Happiness and economics. Princeton/Oxford: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., & Beatton, T. (2012). The mystery of the U-shaped relationship between happiness and age. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 82, 525–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijters, P., Haisken-DeNew, J., & Shields, M. (2004). Money does matter! Evidence from increasing real incomes and life satisfaction in East Germany following reunification. American Economic Review, 94, 730–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaeser, E., Laibson, D., & Sacerdote, B. (2002). An economic approach to social capital. Economic Journal, 112, 437–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, D. P. (1972). The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., & Ruiz Pozuelo, J. (2017). Happiness, stress, and age: How the U curve varies across people and places. Journal of Population Economics, 30, 225–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayo, B., & Seifert, W. (2002). Subjective economic well-being in Eastern Europe. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 329–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapteyn, A., van Praag, B. M. S., & van Herwaarden, F. G. (1978). Individual welfare functions and social reference spaces. Economics Letters, 1, 173–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kassenböhmer, S. C., & Haisken-DeNew, J. (2012). Heresy or enlightenment? The well-being age U-shape effect is flat. Economics Letters, 117, 235–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, H.-P., Behrman, J., & Skytthe, A. (2005). Partner + children = happiness? The effects of partnerships and fertility on well-being. Population and Development Review, 31, 407–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Re-examining adaptation and the setpoint model of happiness: Reaction to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set-point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 8–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1333–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, A. (2005). Change in life satisfaction during adulthood: Findings from the veterans affairs normative aging study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 189–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Rusting, C. L. (1999). Gender differences in well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, A. J. (1997). Happiness and economic performance. Economic Journal, 107, 1815–1831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollis, N. P. (1968). Reference group re-examined. British Journal of Sociology, 19, 300–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodgers, W. (1982). Trends in reported happiness within demographically defined subgroups, 1957–78. Social Forces, 60, 826–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwandt, H. (2016). Unmet aspirations as an explanation for the age U-shape in wellbeing. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 122, 75–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Deaton, A. (2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(22), 9985–9990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1031–1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Landeghem, B. (2012). A test for the convexity of human well-being over the life cycle: Longitudinal evidence from a 20-year panel. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81, 571–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Praag, B., & Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2004). Happiness quantified. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wunder, C., Wiencierz, A., Schwarze, J., & Küchenhoff, H. (2013). Well-being over the life span: Semiparametric evidence from British and German longitudinal data. Review of Economics and Statistics, 95, 154–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, A., & Easterlin, R. (2006). Happily ever after? Cohabitation, marriage, divorce, and happiness in Germany. Population and Development Review, 32, 511–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Dick Easterlin, Carol Graham, David Halpern, Mike Hagerty, Laurence Hazelrigg, Felicia Huppert, Hendrik Juerges, Nicolai Kristensen, Ken Land, Orsolya Lelkes, Andrew Oswald, Steve Platt, Claudia Senik, Andrew Sharpe, Peter Warr and Rainer Winkelmann for useful discussions. The BHPS data were made available through the ESRC Data Archive. The data were originally collected by the ESRC Research Centre on Micro-social Change at the University of Essex. Neither the original collectors of the data nor the Archive bear any responsibility for the analyses or interpretations presented here.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew E. Clark .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendices

1.1 Appendix A

The 12 questions used to create the GHQ-12 measure appear in the BHPS questionnaire as follows:

  1. 1.

    Here are some questions regarding the way you have been feeling over the last few weeks. For each question please ring the number next to the answer that best suits the way you have felt.

    Have you recently… .

    1. a)

      been able to concentrate on whatever you’re doing?

      Better than usual  1

      Same as usual   2

      Less than usual   3

      Much less than usual 4

      then

    2. b)

      lost much sleep over worry?

    3. e)

      felt constantly under strain?

    4. f)

      felt you couldn’t overcome your difficulties?

    5. i)

      been feeling unhappy or depressed?

    6. j)

      been losing confidence in yourself?

    7. k)

      been thinking of yourself as a worthless person?

      with the responses:

      Not at all      1

      No more than usual 2

      Rather more than usual 3

      Much more than usual 4

      then

    8. c)

      felt that you were playing a useful part in things?

    9. d)

      felt capable of making decisions about things?

    10. g)

      been able to enjoy your normal day-to-day activities?

    11. h)

      been able to face up to problems?

    12. i)

      been feeling reasonably happy, all things considered?

      with the responses:

      More so than usual 1

      About same as usual 2

      Less so than usual  3

      Much less than usual 4

1.2 Appendix B

Table 17.4 The distribution of well-being in the BHPS (Inverted Caseness index of the GHQ-12)
Table 17.5 The distribution of well-being in the BHPS (life satisfaction)

1.3 Appendix C

Fig. 17.3
figure 3

GHQ fixed effects by year of birth

Fig. 17.4
figure 4

Life satisfaction fixed effects by year of birth

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Clark, A.E. (2019). Born to Be Mild? Cohort Effects Don’t (Fully) Explain Why Well-Being Is U-Shaped in Age. In: Rojas, M. (eds) The Economics of Happiness. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-15835-4_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics