Skip to main content

How Just Is Justice? Ask a Psycholinguist

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Grammatical Approaches to Language Processing

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 48))

  • 385 Accesses

Abstract

You are a member of a jury. After the trial, the judge reads you and your fellow jurors a set of instructions. One of them begins: Failure of recollection is common. Innocent misrecollection is not uncommon… Confused? Now imagine that your native language is not English or that you never finished high school. Or both. Our justice system depends on jurors making informed decisions to reach a verdict, so when jury instructions are too challenging, jurors not only disengage but return misinformed verdicts. Courtroom practices make jurors’ jobs even harder. Many states don’t provide copies of the instructions and some don’t permit jurors to take notes. Can we make instructions easier for jurors, and in so doing, improve justice? In two studies, we show that jury instruction comprehension significantly improves (a) when subjects read the texts of the instructions while listening to them and (b) when the instructions are rewritten in Plain English, minimizing two linguistic factors: passive verbs and unfamiliar legal expressions, or “legalese”. Improvements were even greater for Study 2’s MTurk subjects than Study 1’s undergraduates. Since these new subjects are closer demographically to jurors, this new data provides even more evidence that current jury instructions need to be rewritten. Taken together, the studies lay the groundwork for reform, psycholinguistics providing judiciaries with the evidence that they need to implement change.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In 1996 the Blue Ribbon Commission on Jury System Improvement stated that “jury instructions as presently given in California and elsewhere are, on occasion, simply impenetrable to the ordinary juror.” In response to the commission’s recommendation, the Judicial Council created the Task Force on Jury Instructions in 1997.

  2. 2.

    An N-gram is a contiguous sequence of n items from a given sample of text or speech. The Google Ngram Viewer or Google Books Ngram Viewer is an online search engine that charts frequencies of any set of comma-delimited search strings using a yearly count of n-grams found in sources printed between 1500 and 2008. See https://books.google.com/ngrams.

  3. 3.

    Mistakes is found with the base form mistake using the INF function, which provides a combined frequency for a word and its inflected forms.

  4. 4.

    The Corpus of Contemporary English database contains more than 560 million words of text (20 million words each year 1990–2017) equally divided among spoken, fiction, popular magazines, newspapers, and academic texts. See https://corpus.byu.edu/coca/.

  5. 5.

    For example, the written version of the jury instructions in the state criminal trial of Michael Jackson spanned ninety-eight pages (Broder, 2005).

  6. 6.

    This is a project of the National Center for State Courts. The full data set includes the number of respondents and the percentages for each state. In contrast to the US Legal report, there was no state reporting that 0% of jurors said that they could take notes.

References

  • American Bar Association, & American Jury Project. (2005). Principles for juries and jury trials. American Bar Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever T. G. (1970). The influence of speech performance on linguistic structure. In G. B. Flores d’Arcais & W. J. M. Levelt (Eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics. (pp. 4–30). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benson, R. W. (1985). The end of legalese: The game is over. NYU Review of Law and Social Change, 13(3), 519–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boch, F., & Piolat, A. (2005). Note taking and learning: A summary of research. The Writing Across the Curriculum Journal, 16, 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1086.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broda-Bahm, K. (2016). Let the jurors take and review notes. The Persuasive Litigator. Retrieved February 14, 2018, from http://www.persuasivelitigator.com/2016/10/let-the-jurors-take-and-review-notes.html.pdf.

  • Broder, J. M. (2005, June 12). Instructions, all 98 pages, may be slowing Jackson jury. The New York Times. Retrieved February 14, 2018, from https://www.nytimes.com/2005/06/12/us/instructions-all-98-pages-may-be-slowing-jackson-jury.html.

  • Brown, R., Waring, R., & Donkaewbua, S. (2008). Incidental vocabulary acquisition from reading, reading-while-listening, and listening to stories. Reading in a Foreign Language, 20(2), 136–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • California., In Richards, P. H., & California. (1969). California jury instructions, civil: Book of approved jury instructions (BAJI). St. Paul: West Pub. Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, A. C. (2009). Gains to L2 listeners from reading while listening vs. listening only in comprehending short stories. System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 37(4), 652–663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charrow, R. P., & Charrow, V. R. (1979). Making legal language understandable: a psycholinguistic study of jury instructions. Columbia Law Review, 79(7), 1306–1374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor, J. (2004, February 25). Jurors need to have their own copies of instructions. The Los Angeles Daily Journal, 7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dąbrowska, E., & Street, J. (2006). Individual differences in language attainment: Comprehension of passive sentences by native and non-native English speakers. Language Sciences, 28(6), 604–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dann, B. M., & Logan, G., III. (1996). Jury reform: The Arizona experience. Judicature, 79(5), 280–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, S., Murphy, B., & Rose, M. R. (2012). The “kettleful of law” in real jury deliberations: Success, failures and next steps. Northwestern Law Review, 106(4), 1573–1608.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, S. (2003). Truth, justice and the jury. Harvard Journal of Law & Public Policy, 26, 143–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, S., & Levi, J. (1996). Improving decisions on death by revising and testing jury instructions. Judicature, 79, 224–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diana, R. A., & Reder, L. M. (2006). The low-frequency encoding disadvantage: Word frequency affects processing demands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 805–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffelmeyer, F. A. (1979). The effect of rewriting prose material on reading comprehension. Reading World, 19(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Einstein, G. O., Morris, J., & Smith, S. (1985). Note-taking, individual differences, and memory for lecture information. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 522–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elwork, A., Sales, B., & Alfini, J. (1982). Making jury instructions understandable. Charlottesville, VA: Michie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 164–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heuer, L., & Penrod, S. D. (1989). Instructing jurors: A field experiment with written and preliminary instructions. Law and Human Behavior, 13(4), 409–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judicial Council of California Civil Jury Instructions (CACI 2003, updated, 2018: http://www.courts.ca.gov/partners/317.htm). Accessed 25 April 2018.

  • Just, M. A., & Clark, H. H. (1973). Drawing inferences from the presuppositions and implications of affirmative and negative sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1976). Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 441–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274(5284), 114–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kassin, S. M., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1979). On the requirements of proof: The timing of judicial instruction and mock juror verdicts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(10), 1877.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiewra, K. A. (1984). The relationship between notetaking over an extended period and actual course-related achievement. College Student Journal, 17, 381–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klare, G. R. (1963). The measurement of readability. Ames, Iowa: Iowa State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marder, N. S. (2005). Bringing jury instructions into the twenty-first century. Notre Dame Law Review, 81, 449–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marder, N. S. (2009). Answering jurors’ questions: next steps in Illinois. Loyola University of Chicago Law Journal, 41, 727–752.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Chomsky, N. (1963). Finitary models of language users. New York: Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. (2002). Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: When reading helps listening. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 156–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop note taking. Psychological science, 25(6), 1159–1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R., & Filby, N. (1972). On the comprehension of active and passive sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 361–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piolat, A., Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2004). Cognitive effort of note taking. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sand, L. B. et al. (2005). Modern federal jury instructions, Criminal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Rothkopf, E. Z. (1984). Contextual enrichment and distribution of practice in the classroom. Cognition and Instruction, 1(3), 341–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spyridakis, J. H., & Isakson, C. S. (1998). Nominalizations vs denominalizations: do they influence what readers recall? Journal of Technical Writing & Communication, 28(2), 163–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thalheimer, W. (2006, February). Spacing learning events over time: what the research says. Retrieved February 22, 2018, from http://www.work-learning.com/catalog/.pdf.

  • Thorley, C. (2016). Note taking and note reviewing enhance jurors’ recall of trial information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30, 655–663. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiersma, P. M. (2009). Communicating with juries: How to draft more understandable jury instructions. Loyola-LA Legal Studies Paper No. 2009-44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiersma, P. M. (2001). The rocky road to legal reform: Improving the language of jury instructions. Brooklyn Law Review, 66, 1081–1118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiersma, P. M. (1993). Reforming the language of jury instructions. Hofstra Law Review, 22, 37–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiersma, P. M. (1999). Jury instructions in the new millennium. Court Review, 36, 28–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Meter, P., Yokoi, L., & Pressley, M. (1994). College students’ theory of note-taking derived from their perceptions of note-taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 323–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2012). Distributing learning over time: the spacing effect in children’s acquisition and generalization of science concepts. Child Development, 83(4), 1137–1144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1972). Thinking and reasoning: Selected readings. Harmondworth: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R. L., & Eggert, A. C. (2002). Notetaking in college classes: student patterns and instructional strategies. The Journal of General Education, 51(3), 173–199.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to the Massachusetts Bar Association for their initial interest in this project, for sponsoring me as a Research Fellow and awarding me and my team a small grant to get our research going. Since then, we have benefitted from a grant from the Northeastern University Humanities Center and from a series of student research and travel grants from the Provost’s office and the College of Social Science and Humanities. In addition, many individuals played roles in many aspects of this research and to them I extend my personal thanks: the Honorable Gabrielle Wolohojian, Associate Justice, Massachusetts Appeals Court, the Honorable Judith Fabricant, Chief Justice, Massachusetts Superior Court, the Honorable Francis Fecteau, Associate Justice, Massachusetts Appeals Court (Emeritus), Jack McDevitt, Northeastern University Associate Dean, College of Social Sciences and Humanities, and Jeremy Paul, former Dean, Northeastern University School of Law. But my largest debt is to my hard-working research team. These students have earned my deepest thanks: for their dedication to our work, for their comments, suggestions, and thought-provoking ideas, and for keeping things going through thick and thin. This year’s team members were (in alphabetical order): Kathryn Aucella, Leah Butz, Julien Cherry, Avery Isaacs, Shaughnessy Jones, Samantha Laureano, Abbie MacNeal, Matthew Monjarrez, Francielle Reis, Benjamin Rubin, Rachel Smith, and Yian Xu. I am also grateful to our alumni, on whose shoulders our current team is working. A special shout-out goes to two recent ones, Katherine Fiallo and Alexander Jones, for holding up the legal side of this project. And though they all have been invaluable in the work presented here, all errors and omissions are mine.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janet Randall .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix

Appendix

Current and Plain English versions of a sample instruction, Standard of Proof, and its corresponding questions

After reading each question, circle either T for True or F for False

 

True

False

1.

A “preponderance of the evidence” means a slow, careful pondering of the evidence

T

F

2.

A “preponderance of the evidence” is the standard of proof used in civil cases

T

F

3.

The greater weight of the evidence is all that is meant by a “preponderance of the evidence”

T

F

4.

In a civil case, it is the defendant who must meet the burden of proof

T

F

5.

In a civil case, the burden of proof is met only if there is proof beyond a reasonable doubt

T

F

6.

If you believe that the preponderance of the evidence supports the plaintiff but you still have some doubts, you must decide in favor of the defendant

T

F

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Randall, J. (2019). How Just Is Justice? Ask a Psycholinguist. In: Carlson, K., Clifton, Jr., C., Fodor, J. (eds) Grammatical Approaches to Language Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 48. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-01563-3_15

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-01563-3_15

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-01562-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-01563-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics