Skip to main content

Knowledge as Embodied Performance

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Human-Computer Interaction Series ((HCIS))

Abstract

This chapter presents an analysis of knowledge as a process of tacit knowing embodied in dialogue rather than as a process of information transfer embodied in signal processing (Shannon and Weaver 1949). Within dialogue we acquire knowledge (experiential knowledge, tacit knowing, practical knowledge) through our embodied performance of ’how ’ and ’what’ we know about the communication situation we are in. This embodied performance includes utterances, gestures, movement and modulation of body and voice, as well as mediating artefacts, such as interactive technologies. This chapter explores the implications of this perspective of knowledge for the conception and design of interactive technologies that seek to facilitate joint action and joint attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alibali, M.W. & Heath, D.C. (1999), Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: some gestures are meant to be seen. Source of reference was this pre-published paper. (Later publication of this article: Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001) in Journal of Memory and Language, Vol. 44:169-188.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, J. and Core, M. (1997). DAMSL: Dialog Act Markup in Several Layers. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allwood, J., Nivre, J., & Ahlsen, E. (1991). On the Semantics and Pragmatics of Linguistic Feedback. Technical Report No. 64. Gothenburg Papers on Theoretical Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M.L. (2003) Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence 149(1): 91-130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arom, S. (1991). African Polyphony and Polyrhythm: musical structure and methodology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1955). “The Message. ’This is the Play.”’ In Schaffner, B. (ed.) Group Processes. Vol.II. New York: Macy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J.B. (1994). Gestures as Part of Speech: Methodological Implications. Research on Language and Social Interaction. 27(3): 201-221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J.B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., Rose, L. (1995). Gestures Specialized for Dialogue. In PSPB, 21(4): 394-405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekerian, D.A. and Dennett, J.L. (1990). ‘Spoken and Written Recall of Visual Narratives’, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 175-187

    Google Scholar 

  • Birdwhistle, R.L. (1970). Kinesics and Context. University of Pennsylvania

    Google Scholar 

  • Blacking, J. (1976) How Musical is Man? London: Faber

    Google Scholar 

  • Carletta, J., Isard, A., Isard S., Doherty-Sneddon, G, & Anderson, A. (1997). The Reliability of a Dialogue Structure Coding System. Association for Computational Linguistics. 23(1):13-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassell, J., Sullivan,J., Prevost, S., Churchill, E. (2000) Embodied Conversational Agents. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H. and Schaefer, E.F. (1989). Contributing to Discourse. Cognitive Science. 13:259-294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H.H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, M., Sager, R., and Will, U. (2005). In Time with the Music: The Concept of Entrainment and its Significance for Ethnomusicology. In European Meetings in Ethnomusicology II, ESEM CounterPoint Vol.1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1966). Sound Film Analysis of Normal and Pathological Behavior Patterns. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143:338-347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, I. (2007 in press). The Evolutionary Nature of Musical Meaning. In Musica Scientiae

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Paola, S. (17th April, 2001) Gesture and Narrative Creation in Avatar-Based 3D Virtual Communities). Invited paper at Gesture and Dialogue Seminar, CSLI, Stanford University. http://www.dipaola.org/sig99/sld002.htm

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. (1969). The Repertoire of Nonverbal Behaviour: Categories, Origins, Usage, and Coding. In Semiotica. 1:49-98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. (1972). Hand Movements. In Journal of Communication. Vol. 22: 353-374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., Roessler, J. (2005). Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds. Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R.A. 1998. Not Channels But Composite Signals: Speech, Gesture, Diagrams and Object Demonstrations Are Integrated in Multimodal Explanations. In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 321-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari P.F., Fogassi L., Gallese V., Rizzolatti G. (2003) Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17 (8):1703-1714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F. (1997). The Development of Meaning in Infancy: Early Communication and Social Understanding. In S. Hala (Ed.) The Development of Social Cognition. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A.J. 1991. Sociality of Solitary Smiling: Potentiation by an Implicit Audience. In Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60.2: 229-240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill, J.H. (2000). The Tacit Mode. Michael Polanyi’s Postmodern Philosophy. New York: SUNY Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, K.S. (2007) (in this volume). Rethinking the Interaction Architecture. In SP Gill (Ed.) Cognition, Communication and Interaction. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (1995) Dialogue and Tacit Knowledge for Knowledge Transfer. PhD Thesis, University of Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (1996) Aesthetic Design: Dialogue and Learning. A Case Study of Landscape Architecture. AI & Society, Vol. 9(3).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (1998). Body Language: The unspoken dialogue of Bodies in Rhythm. In Proceedings of the ESSLI Workshop on Mutual Knowledge, Common Ground and Public Information.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P., Kawamori, M., Katagiri, Y., Shimojima, A. (2000). The Role of Body Moves in Dialogue. In RASK, Vol.12: 89-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (2002). The Parallel Coordinated Move: Case of a Conceptual Drawing Task. Published Working Paper: CKIR, Helsinki. ISBN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P., and Kawamori, M. (2002). Coordination of Gestures in a Non Face-to-Face Setting. In M. Rector, I. Poggi & N. Trigo (Eds.) Gestures, Meaning and Use. Porto: Fundacao Fernando Pessoa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. and Borchers, J.O. (2004). Knowledge in Co-Action: Social Intelligence in Collaborative Design Activity. AI & Society, 17(3).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (2004). Body Moves and Tacit Knowing. In Gorayska, B. and Mey, J.L. (Eds.) Cognition and Technology. John Benjamin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gill, S.P. (2005) Pulse Periodicity in Paralinguistic Coordination. Presented at International Conference, Interacting Bodies, Lyon, 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Good, D.A. (1996). Pragmatics and Presence. AI & Society Journal. Vol.10 (3&4):309-14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1997). The Blackness of Black: Colour Categories as Situated Practice. In Lauren B. Resnick, Roger Saljo, Clotilde Pontecorvo, & Barbara Burge (Eds.), Discourse, Tools and Reasoning: Essays on Situated Cognition (pp.111-140). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goranzon, B. (1993). The Practical Intellect: Computers and Skills (Artificial Intelligence and Society). Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guberina, P. (1985). The role of the body in learning foreign languages. Revue de Phonetique Applique: 37-50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guimbretiere, F., Stone, M., Winograd, T. (2001). Stick it on the Wall: A Metaphor for Interaction with Large Displays. Submitted to Computer Graphics (SIGGRAPH 2001 Proceedings).

    Google Scholar 

  • Himberg, T. (2006). Co-operative tapping and collective time-keeping - differences of timing accuracy in duet performance with human or computer partner. Presentation at 9th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Bologna, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the Wild. MIT Press: Masachusetts

    Google Scholar 

  • Ikuta, K. (1990) The Role of ‘Craft Language’ in Learning Waza. AI & Society, Vol. 4(2): 127-147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, H. and Arita, K., (1991). ClearFace: Translucent Multiuser Interface for TeamWorkStation, In Proceedings of Second European Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work (ECSCW ’91), Amsterdam, 25-27 September. pp. 163-174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, H. and Kobayashi, M. (1992). ClearBoard: A Seamless Media for Shared Drawing and Conversation with Eye-Contact. In Proceedings of Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’92), ACM SIGCHI, Monterey, pp. 525-532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, H., Kobayashi, M. and Arita, K. (1994). Iterative Design of Seamless Collaboration Media,” Communications of the ACM (CACM), Special Issue on Internet Technology, ACM, Vol. 37, No. 8.pp. 83-97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, Marc. 1997. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Action. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johannessen, K.S. (1988) Rule Following and Tacit Knowledge. AI & Society, Vol. 2(4): 287-303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawamori, M., Shimazu, A., Kogure, K. (1994). Roles of Interjectory Utterances in Spoken Discourse. Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawamori, M., Kawabata, T., Shimazu, A. (1998). Discourse Markers in Spontaneous Dialogue: A corpus based study of Japanese and English. In Proceedings of 17th International Conference on Computational Linguistics (COLING-ACL98).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1970). Movement Coordination in Social Interaction: Some examples described. In Acta Psychologia, 32:100-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1972) Some Relationships Between Body Motion and Speech: An analysis of an example. In A. Seigman and B. Pope (eds.) Studies in Dyadic Communication. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1990) Conducting Interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S. (1993). Language and thought interface: A study of spontaneous gestures and Japanese mimetics. Ph.D. thesis. Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Krogh, G., Ichijo, K., Nonaka, I. (2000). Enabling Knowledge Creation: How to Unlock the Mystery of Tacit Knowledge and Release the Power of Innovation. OUP: Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Large, E. and Jones, M.R. (1999) The Dynamics of Attending: How People Track Time-Varying Events. Pyschological Review. Vol. 106(1): 119-159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. and Varela, F. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realisation of the Living. Dordecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping together in time. London: Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Mey, J. (2001). Pragmatics. An Introduction. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miall, D. S., & Dissanayake, E. (2003). “The Poetics of Babytalk.” Human Nature 14: 337-364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D., Cassell, J., McCullough, K-E. (1994). Communicative Effects of Speech-Mismatched Gestures. Research on Language and Social Interaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1964). Personal Knowledge: Towards a post critical philosophy. N.Y.: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1966). The Tacit Dimension. Doubleday. Reprinted version, 1983, Gloucester, Mass.: Peter Smith.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1984) Computation and Cognition. Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiner, M. and Gilbert, J. (2004) The Symbiotic Roles of Empirical Experimentation and Thought Experimentation in the Learning of Physics. International Journal of Science Education, 26:1819-1834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G., Fogassi L., Gallese V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2: 661-670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saha, B. and Gangopadhyay, S. (2006) Building a Pedagogy around Action and Emotion: Experiences of Blind Opera of Kolkata. AI & Society, Vol. 21: 57-71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheflen, A.E. (1974). How Behaviour Means. Exploring the contexts of speech and meaning: Kinesics, posture, interaction, setting, and culture. New York: Anchor Press/Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffrin, D. (1987). Discourse Markers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sha, X.W. (2002) Resistance Is Fertile: Gesture and Agency in the Field of Responsive Media, in Makeover: Writing the Body into the Posthuman Technoscape, Configurations, 10(3): 439-472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sha, X-W and Gill, S.P. (2005). Gesture and Response in Field-Based Performance. In the ACM Proceedings of Creativity and Cognition 2005, Goldsmiths College, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Sha, X.W. (2005) “The TGarden Performance Research Project.” In Modern Drama, Special Issue: Technology. 48(3):585-608.

    MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. and Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana, Il: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimojima, A., Katagiri, Y., Koiso, H. (1997). Scorekeeping for Conversation-Construction. In Proceedings of the Munich Workshop on Semantics and a Pragmatics of Dialogue

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (Ed.) (1982) Models of bounded rationality. Behavioral economics and business organization, 2: 424-443. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1969). The sciences of the artificial (1st ed). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1983). Reason in human affairs. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. and Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stobart, H. and Cross, I. (2000) The Andean Anacrusis? rhythmic structure and perception in Easter songs of Northern Potosí, Bolivia. British Journal of Ethnomusicology, 9(2): 63-94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolbert, E. (2001) Music and Meaning: An Evolutionary Story. In Psychology of Music, Vol. 29, No. 1, 84-94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilghman, B.R. (1988). Seeing and Seeing-As. AI & Society Journal, Vol.2(4): 303-319 London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28: 675-691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C., Aitken K.J. (1994): Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. In: Development and Psychopathology, 6: 597-633. (zit. nach Trevarthen, 1996)

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2005). “Disorganized rhythm and synchrony: Early signs of autism and Rett syndrome". In Brain & Development, 27: S25-S34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2005). “First things first: infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language". In Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 31(1):91-113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weizenbaum, J. (1966) ELIZA–A Computer Program For the Study of Natural Language Communication Between Man and MachineCommunications of the ACM. Volume 9(1) (January 1966): 36-35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer Power and Human Reason. San Francisco: WH Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2008 Springer-Verlag London Limited

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gill, S.P. (2008). Knowledge as Embodied Performance. In: Gill, S. (eds) Cognition, Communication and Interaction. Human-Computer Interaction Series. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84628-927-9_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84628-927-9_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-84628-926-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-84628-927-9

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics