Skip to main content

Dynamic Information Is Integral to Perception, Cognition, and Emotion

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 566 Accesses

Abstract

The human brain and its sensory systems are evolutionarily designed to be sensitive to movement and change and dynamic stimuli (Cacioppo & Freberg, 2016). Moreover, our higher-order perceptions of the world and thinking abilities appear to have their roots in perceptual systems that are sensitive to dynamic information (Fodor, 1972; Freyd, 1987; Shepard, 1981; Shepard & Podgorny, 1978). People, like most other animals, use dynamic information to increase their odds of survival in an often-dangerous environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adler, J. M., Turner, A. F., Brookshier, K. M., Monahan, C., Walder-Biesanz, I., Harmeling, L. H., et al. (2015). Variation in narrative identity is associated with trajectories of mental health over several years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 476–496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038601

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, E., & Linder, D. (1965). Gain and loss of esteem as determinants of interpersonal attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1(2), 156–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(65)90043-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2002). Being there conceptually: Simulating categories in preparation for situated action. In N. L. Stein, P. J. Bauer, & M. Rabinowitz (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 1–15). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2003a). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 358, 1177–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2003b). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 513–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Bratslavsky, E. (1999). Passion, intimacy, and time: Passionate: Love as a function of change in intimacy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(1), 49–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (Ed.), Adaptation level theory: A symposium. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917–927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2008). Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodiment grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 187–207). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Wagner, B. (2009). Body posture effects on self-evaluation: A self-validation approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1053–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18, 1–21 http://www.jstor.org/stable/1343711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Bernston, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determination of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J., & Freberg, L. (2016). Discovering psychology: The science of mind (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21, 1363–1368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stress. NeuroImage, 12, 478–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doerrfeld, A., & Shiffrar, M. (2011). Representational momentum varies across objects. Journal of Vision, 11, 731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stem, L., & Van Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, K. (1998). The face of time: Temporal cues in facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Science, 9, 270–276. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00054

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1972). Some reflections on L. S. Vygotsky’s “thought and language”. Cognition, 1, 83–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J. (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, 94, 427–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., Panzer, T. M., & Chang, J. L. (1988). Representing statics as forces in equilibrium. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 395–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1997). Mental models, space, and embodied cognition. In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith, & J. Vaid (Eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (Vol. xv , 567pp., pp. 495–522). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10227-018

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 Years of cognitive psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 573–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsee, C. K., & Abelson, R. P. (1991). The velocity relation: Satisfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 341–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (1995). Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 322–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (1997). Target size and displacement along the axis of implied gravitational attraction: Effects of implied weight and evidence of representational gravity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1484–1493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822–851. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196775

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D., & Schubert, T. W. (2009). Weight as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20, 1169–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, Y., Williams, L. E., Clark, M. S., Gray, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 507–515. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq077

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(4), 475–486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0036125.

  • Lakoff, G. (2015, March 14). How the brain thinks: The embodiment hypothesis. Retrieved from https://youtu.be/WuUnMCq-ARQ

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1045–1067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003066X.46.8.819

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21, 222–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359333

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nagle, E. (1961). The structure of science: Problems in the logic of scientific explanation. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1969). On the whys and wherefores of E, P, and A. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 194–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027715

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (2007). The child’s conception of the world. New York: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, C. L., & Farah, M. J. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 334–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H. (1983). Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition, 2, 52–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: Guiding and self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 479–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., & Gotay, C. C. (1982). Physical posture: Could it have regulatory or feedback effects on motivation and emotion? Motivation and Emotion, 6, 273–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00992249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1981). Psychophysical complementarity. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp. 279–341). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., & Podgorny, P. (1978). Cognitive processes that resemble perceptual processes. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (pp. 189–237). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slepian, M. L., Wesbuch, M., Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2012). Proprioception and person perception: Politicians and professors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1621–1628. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212457786

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spinoza, B. (1996). Ethics. New York: Penguin Classics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, E., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the imagination: Mental stimulation, self-regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In H. S. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness (pp. 3–56). New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195161564.003.0001 364 pp.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of over head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp0103_2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1257–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Riskind, J.H., Rector, N.A. (2018). Dynamic Information Is Integral to Perception, Cognition, and Emotion. In: Looming Vulnerability. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-8782-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics