Skip to main content

What Is Fluency?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Fluency Construct

Abstract

Fluency is a ubiquitous, but a complicated term. This chapter provides an overview of various conceptions of fluency across a range of disciplines and research contexts as well as discusses implications for research, assessment, and instruction. We include an overview of definitions and exemplars of fluency in language use, reading, and mathematics followed by an exploration of fluency concepts (e.g., automaticity, efficiency, and procedural skill) and considerations for research. The chapter concludes with recommendations for investigations of fluency including attending to measurement issues, explicitly defining and analyzing discrete components of fluency, and leveraging these precise definitions to evaluate the interactions between task and proficiency inherent to fluency exercises.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Interestingly, Samuels has since become one of the more vocal proponents of the importance of not only prosody, but also comprehension in the definition and measurement of oral reading ­fluency (e.g., Samuels, Ediger, & Fautsch-Patridge, 2005; Samuels, 2007).

  2. 2.

    Hasher and Zacks (1979) argue that automaticity should not be applied to processes that benefit from practice, but this perspective is decidedly in the minority (for discussion see Fisk & Schneider, 1984).

  3. 3.

    Note that among reading researchers interested in full isolation of decoding, reading of decodable nonsense words out of context is considered preferable to reading real words out of context because it isolates decoding even from sight recognition and effects of vocabulary knowledge (e.g., Ehri, 2005).

References

  • Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing, 19(9), 933–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal. Reading Teacher, 36(6), 556–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1999). Terminology pertaining to fluency and fluency disorders: guidelines (Guidelines). www.asha.org/policy. http://www.asha.org/docs/html/GL1999-00063.html#sthash.7J4aMThZ.dpuf.

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1996). ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. American Psychologist, 51(4), 355–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, D. L., Biancarosa, G., Park, B. J., Bousselot, T., Smith, J., Baker, S. K., Kame’enui, E. J., Alonzo, J., & Tindal, G. (in press). Validity of CBM measures of oral reading fluency and reading comprehension on high-stakes reading assessments in Grades 7 and 8. Reading & Writing. doi:10.1007/s11145-014-9505-4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2010). Text complexity and oral reading prosody in young readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 388–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, R. G., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E. B., Groff, C., Kuhn, M. R., & Steiner, L. (2013). A spectrographically grounded scale for evaluating reading expressiveness. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 105–133. doi:10.1002/rrq.43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brasseur-Hock, I. F., Hock, M. F., Kieffer, M. J., Biancarosa, G., & Deshler, D. D. (2011). Adolescent struggling readers in urban schools: Results of a latent class analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 438–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). Children’s arithmetical difficulties: Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buly, M. R., & Valencia, S. W. (2002). Below the bar: Profiles of students who fail state reading assessments. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24(3), 219–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnine, D. (1997). Instructional design in mathematics for students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(2), 130–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90054-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 25–40). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, F. (1997). What do we mean by fluency? System, 25(4), 535–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chard, D. J., Clarke, B., Baker, S., Otterstedt, J., Braun, D., & Katz, R. (2005). Using measures of number sense to screen for difficulties in mathematics: Preliminary findings. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 30(2), 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P. (2005). How reading research can inform mathematics difficulties: The search for the core deficit. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 313–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, B., & Shinn, M. (2004). A preliminary investigation into the identification and development of early mathematics curriculum-based measurement. School Psychology Review, 33, 234–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, B., Nelson, N., & Shanley, L. (in-press). Mathematics fluency: more than the weekly timed test. In K. D. Cummings & Y. Petscher (Eds.), Fluency metrics in education: Implications for test developers, researchers, and practitioners. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowie, R., Douglas-Cowie, E., & Wichmann, A. (2002). Prosodic characteristics of skilled reading: Fluency and expressiveness in 8-10-year-old readers. Language and Speech, 45(1), 47–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daane, M. C., Campbell, J. R., Grigg, W. S., Goodman, M. J., & Oranje, A. (2005). Fourth-grade students reading aloud: NAEP 2002 special study of oral reading (NCES 2006—469). U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deno, S. L., Mirkin, P. K., & Chiang, B. (1982). Identifying valid measures of reading. Exceptional Children, 49, 36–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derwing, T. M., Rossiter, M. J., Munro, M. J., & Thomson, R. I. (2004). Second language fluency: Judgments on different tasks. Language Learning, 54(4), 655–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deschuyteneer, M., & Vandierendonck, A. (2005). The role of response selection and input monitoring in solving simple arithmetical products. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1472–1483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Espin, C.A., & Deno, S.L. (in-press). Oral reading fluency or reading aloud from text: An analysis through a unified view of construct validity. In K. D. Cummings & Y. Petscher (Eds.). Fluency metrics in education: implications for test developers, researchers, and practitioners. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farukh, A., & Vulchanova, M. (2014). Predictors of reading in Urdu: Does deep orthography have an impact? Dyslexia, 20(2), 146–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. J. (1979). On fluency. In C. Fillmore, D. Kempler, & W. S.-Y. Wang (Eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (pp. 85–101). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisk, A. D., & Schneider, W. (1984). Memory as a function of attention, level of processing, and automatization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(2), 181–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Floyd, R., Evans, J. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2003). Relations between measures of Cathill-Horn-Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and mathematics achievement across the school-age years. Psychology in the Schools, 40, 155–171. doi:10.1002/pits.10083

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fluency. (n.d.). In Oxford Dictionaries English online dictionary. http://www.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/english/fluency.

  • Foster, P., & Skehan, P. (1996). The influence of planning and task type on second language performance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(3), 299–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, P., & Skehan, P. (2013). Anticipating a post-task activity: The effects on accuracy, complexity, and fluency of second language performance. Canadian Modern Language Review, 69(3), 249–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Deno, S. L., & Marsten, D. (1983). Improving the reliability of curriculum-based measures of academic skills for psychoeducational decision making. Diagnostique, 8, 135–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 239–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 345–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C., Brown, S. C., & Samaranayake, V. A. (1991). Cognitive addition: A short longitudinal study of strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 787–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 293–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gojak, L. M. (2012, Nov 1). Fluency: Simply fast and accurate? I think not! National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. http://www.nctm.org/about/content.aspx?id=34791

  • Goodman, K. S. (1973). The 13th easy way to make learning to read difficult: A reaction to Gleitman and Rozin. Reading Research Quarterly, 8(4), 484–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, C., & Shinn, M. R. (2003). Characteristics of word callers: An investigation of the accuracy of teachers’ judgments of reading comprehension and oral reading skills. School Psychology Review, 32(2), 228–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselbring, T. S., Goin, L. I., & Bransford, J. D. (1988). Developing math automaticity in learning handicapped children: The role of computerized drill and practice. Focus on Exceptional Children, 20(6), 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, B., & Spatriu, A. (2008). The influence of self-efficacy and metacognitve prompting on math problem-solving efficiency. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(4), 875–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Housen, A., & Kuiken, F. (2009). Complexity, accuracy and fluency (CAF) in second language acquisition research (special issue). Applied Linguistics, 30(4), 461–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (2012). Complexity, accuracy, and fluency: Definitions, measurement, and research. In A. Housen, F. Kuiken, & I. Vedder (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency: Complexity, accuracy, and fluency in SLA (pp. 1–20). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R. F., Pullen, P. C., Lane, H. B., & Torgesen, J. K. (2009). The complex nature of reading fluency: A multidimensional view. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(1), 4–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C.A., & Deno, S.L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 719–729.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Palmer, J. (1985). Assessing automaticity. Acta Psychologica, 60(2–3), 157–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kershaw, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). A latent variable approach to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 433–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauda, S. L., & Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 310–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future directions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 367–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89(5), 483–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. G., & Meltzer, R. H. (1976). Visual rhythms: Report on a method for facilitating the teaching of reading. Journal of Literacy Research, 8(2), 53–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meisinger, E. B., Bradley, B. A., Schwanenflugel, P. J., Kuhn, M. R., & Morris, R. D. (2009). Myth and reality of the word caller: The relation between teacher nominations and prevalence among elementary school children. School Psychology Quarterly, 24(3), 147–159.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meisinger, E. B., Bradley, B. A., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Kuhn, M. R. (2010). Teachers’ perceptions of word callers and related literacy concepts. School Psychology Review, 39(1), 54–68.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2008). A longitudinal study of reading prosody as a dimension of oral reading fluency in early elementary school children. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(4), 336–354.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nathan, R. G., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). The causes and consequences of differences in reading fluency. Theory into Practice, 30(3), 176–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2009). Towards an organic approach to investigating CAF in instructed SLA: The case of complexity. Applied Linguistics, 30(4), 555–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pallotti, G. (2009). CAF: Defining, refining and differentiating constructs. Applied Linguistics, 30(4), 590–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppanen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A.-M., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Developmental links to very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes: Strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(4), 353–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rasinski, T., Rikli, A., & Johnston, S. (2009). Reading fluency: More than automaticity? More than a concern for the primary grades? Literacy Research & Instruction, 48(4), 350–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhymer, K. N., Dittmer, K. I., Skinner, C. H., & Jackson, B. (2000). Effectiveness of a multi-component treatment for improving mathematics fluency. School Psychology Quarterly, 15(1), 40–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rindermann, H., & Neubauer, A. C. (2004). Processing speed, intelligence, creativity, and school performance: Testing of causal hypotheses using structural equation models. Intelligence, 32, 573–589. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (1979). The method of repeated readings. Reading Teacher, 32, 403–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (2007). The DIBELS tests: Is speed of barking at print what we mean by reading fluency?: Commentary on “The relation between DIBELS, reading comprehension, and vocabulary in urban first-grade students.” Reading Research Quarterly, 42(4), 563–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J., Ediger, K.-A., & Fautsch-Patridge, T. (2005). The importance of fluent reading. New England Reading Association Journal, 41(1), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. (1992). Psychological mechanisms underlying second language fluency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14(4), 357–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreiber, P. A. (1980). On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of Reading Behavior, 12(3), 177–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, R. D., Speece, D. L., Harring, J. R., & Ritchey, K. D. (2013). Fluency has a role in the simple view of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17(2), 108–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slocum, T. A., Street, E. M., & Gilberts, G. (1995). A review of research and theory on the relation between oral reading rate and reading comprehension. Journal of Behavioral Education, 5, 377–398. doi: 10.1007/bf02114539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F., & Holmes, D. L. (1971). The independence of letter, word, and meaning identification. Reading Research Quarterly, 6, 394–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R. M., & Thorndike-Christ, T. (2010). Measurement and evaluation in psychology and education (8th ed). Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurlow, R., & van den Broek, P. (1997). Automaticity and inference generation during reading comprehension. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 13(2), 165–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(4), 383–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency (2nd ed). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vagh, S. B., & Biancarosa, G. (2011, July). Early literacy in Hindi: The role of oral reading fluency. Poster presented at the 12th International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Montreal, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, F., & Ellis, R. (2003). The effects of pre-task planning and on-line planning on fluency, complexity and accuracy in L2 monologic oral production. Applied Linguistics, 24(1), 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gina Biancarosa .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Biancarosa, G., Shanley, L. (2016). What Is Fluency?. In: Cummings, K., Petscher, Y. (eds) The Fluency Construct. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2803-3_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics