Skip to main content

Prosocial Behavior and Social Status

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Psychology of Social Status

Abstract

Humans are a very prosocial species, in that we often help others even at a cost to ourselves. Such behavior affects—and is affected by—a person’s social status. In the current chapter, we examine the interactions between social status and prosocial behavior, and we show that causation goes in both directions. On the one hand, laboratory and field evidence show that prosocial behavior can be a means of achieving, or maintaining, elevated status and accessing the accompanying material and social rewards. On the other hand, possessing status can also affect prosocial behavior (for better or worse) by altering the costs and benefits of prosociality, for example, by affecting people’s dependence on others, their vested interest in others, their ability to be prosocial, and their need for status maintenance. Status thus influences the use of prosocial behavior, but can increase it or decrease it depending on the context and the specific type of prosocial behavior. By understanding this bi-directional causation and applying it, we can harness people’s desire for status to promote prosocial behaviors by ensuring its visibility and by fostering competitive altruism.

Authors Sara Kafashan and Adam Sparks contributed equally to this work.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Andreoni, J., & Petrie, R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: A glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1605–1623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, S., & Eckel, C. C. (1998). The economic value of status. Journal of Socio-Economics, 27, 495–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, S., Eckel, C., Grossman, P. J., & Zame, W. (2001). Status in markets. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116, 161–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2004). Trustworthiness and competitive altruism can also solve the “tragedy of the commons”. Evolution & Human Behavior, 25, 209–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2006). Reputational benefits for altruistic punishment. Evolution & Human Behaviour, 27, 344–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2010a). Reputation and the evolution of generous behavior. Hauppage: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2010b). Altruism as a courtship display: Some effects of third-party generosity on audience perceptions. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2011). The evolution of charitable behaviour and the power of reputation. In C. Roberts (Ed.), Applied evolutionary psychology (pp. 149–172). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2012). Harnessing the power of reputation: strengths and limits for promoting cooperative behaviours. Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 868–883.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 164–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P., & Benard, S. (2013). Who cries wolf, and when: Manipulation of perceived threats to preserve rank in cooperative groups. PLoS ONE, 8, e73863.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P., & Reeve, H. K. (2012). The varying relationship between helping and individual quality. Behavioural Ecology, 23, 693–698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P., & Van Vugt, M. (in press). The evolutionary psychology of human pro-sociality: Adaptations, byproducts, and mistakes. In D. Schroeder & W. Graziano (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P., & Willer, R. (2007). Partner choice creates competitive altruism in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 749–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, A. (2001). Social dilemmas and shame-based sanctions: Experimental results from rural Zimbabwe. Working Paper WPS/2001-11, Centre for the Study of African Economies, Oxford, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Sager, K., Garst, E., Kang, M., Rubchinsky, K., & Dawson, K. (1997). Is empathy-induced helping due to self-other merging? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 495–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bixenstine, V., Levitt, C., & Wilson, K. (1966). Collaboration among six persons in a prisoner’s dilemma game. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 10, 488–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourrat, P., Baumard, N., & McKay, R. (2011). Surveillance cues enhance moral condemnation. Evolutionary Psychology, 9, 193–199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (1978). New Guinea: Ecology, society, and culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 7, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, T. C., & Hare, B. (2007). Engineering human cooperation: Does involuntary neural activation increase public goods contributions? Human Nature, 18, 88–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon, N. (1997). Yanomamö (5th ed.). Wadsworth Pub Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of social status. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 31, 334–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A., & Henrich, J. (2013). Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 103–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, J. (2002). Recognizing large donations to public goods: An experimental test. Managerial and Decision Economics, 23, 33–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462, 51–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diekmann, A. (1993). Cooperation in an asymmetric volunteer’s dilemma: Theory and experimental evidence. International Journal of Game Theory, 22, 75–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Fatas, E., & Wilson, R. (2010). Cooperation and status in organizations. Journal of Public Economic Theory, 12, 737–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekström, M. (2011). Do watching eyes affect charitable giving? Evidence from a field experiment. Experimental Economics, 15, 530–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ernest-Jones, M., Nettle, D., & Bateson, M. (2011). Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behavior: A field experiment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 172–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiddick, L., Cummins, D. D., Janicki, M., Lee, S., & Erlich, N. (2013). A cross-cultural study of noblesse oblige in economic decision-making. Human Nature, 24, 318–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francey, D., & Bergmuller, R. (2012). Images of eyes enhance investments in a real-life public good. PLoS ONE, 7, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Miller, G. F., & Kenrick, D. T. (2007). Blatant benevolence and conspicuous consumption: When romantic motives elicit strategic costly signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 85–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Gangestad, S. W., Perea, E. F., Shapiro, J. R., & Kenrick, D. T. (2009). Aggress to impress: Hostility as an evolved context-dependent strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 980–994.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 392–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 26, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 17–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, C., & Van Vugt, M. (2006). Giving for glory in social dilemmas: The competitive altruism hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1402–1413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haselhuhn, M. P., & Wong, E. M. (2012). Bad to the bone: Facial structure predicts unethical behavior. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 571–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & McElreath, R. (2001). In search of Homo Economicus: Behavioural experiments in 15 small scale societies. The American Economic Review, 91, 73–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., & Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 795–855.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesogorol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 1767–1770.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesogorol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 1480–1484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1988). Tradeoffs in male and female reproductive strategies among the Ache: Part 1. In L. Betzig, M. Borgerhoff Mulder, & P. Turke (Eds.), Human reproductive behaviour: A darwinian perspective (pp. 277–289). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kokko, H., Johnstone, R. A., & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2001). The evolution of cooperative breeding through group augmentation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 268, 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, M. W., Piff, P. K., & Keltner, D. (2009). Social class, sense of control, and social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 992–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, M. W., Côté, S., & Keltner, D. (2010). Social class, contextualism, and empathetic accuracy. Psychological Science, 21, 1716–1723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, M. W., Piff, P. K., & Keltner, D. (2011). Social class as culture: The convergence of resources and rank in the social realm. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 246–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R. (2001). The social psychophysics of cooperation: Nonverbal communication in a public goods game. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 241–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahti, D. C., & Weinstein, B. S. (2005). The better angels of our nature: Group stability and the evolution of moral tension. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 47–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebe, U., & Tutic, A. (2010). Status groups and altruistic behaviour in dictator games. Rationality and Society, 22, 353–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mealey, L. (1985). The relationship between social status and biological success: A case study of the Mormon religious hierarchy. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 249–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2010). Altruism toward in-group members as a reputation mechanism. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 109–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milinski, M., Semmann, D., & Krambeck, H.-J. (2002a). Reputation helps solve the “tragedy of the commons”. Nature, 415, 424–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milinski, M., Semmann, D., & Krambeck, H. J. (2002b). Donors to charity gain in both indirect reciprocity and political reputation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269, 881–883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nadler, A., & Halabi, S. (2006). Intergroup helping as status relations: Effects of status stability, identification, and type of help on receptivity to high-status group’s help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 97–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelissen, R. (2008). The price you pay: Cost-dependent reputation effects of altruistic punishment. Evolution & Human Behavior, 29, 242–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D., & Pollet, T. V. (2008). Natural selection on male wealth in humans. The American Naturalist, 172, 658–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D., Harper, Z., Kidson, A., Stone, R., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Bateson, M. (2013). The watching eyes effect in the dictator game: It’s not how much you give, it’s being seen to give something. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 35–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291–1298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oda, R., Niwa, Y., Honma, A., & Hiraishi, K. (2011). An eye-like painting enhances the expectation of a good reputation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 166–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piddocke, S. (1965). The potlatch system of the Southern Kwakiutl: A new perspective. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 21, 244–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piff, P. K., Krauss, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 771–784.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piff, P. K., Stancato, D. M., Côté, S., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Keltner, D. (2012). Higher social class predicts increased unethical behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 4086–4091.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, K. L., Roberts, G., & Nettle, D. (2012). Eye images increase charitable donations: Evidence from an opportunistic field experiment in a supermarket. Ethology, 188, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers, N., Blackman, A., Lyon, T. P., & Narain, U. (2008). Does disclosure reduce pollution? Evidence from India’s Green Rating Project. Discussion Paper, Resources for the Future, RFF 08-38. http://www.rff.org/RFF/Documents/RFF-DP-08-38.pdf. Accessed 24 Oct 2010.

  • Price, M. E. (2003). Pro-community altruism and social status in a Shuar village. Human Nature, 14, 191–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Apicella, C. L., & Cárdenas, R. A. (2012). Masculine voices signal men’s threat potential in forager and industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 601–609.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, J. (4 December 2007). Lexus exceeds LS600h sales target by three hundred percent [Web log post]. http://www.autoblog.com/2007/12/04/lexus-exceeds-ls600h-sales-target-by-three-hundred-percent/.

  • Reeve, H. K., & Hölldobler, B. (2007). The emergence of a superorganism through intergroup competition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 9736–9740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, H. K., & Shen, S.-F. (2006). A missing model in reproductive skew theory—the bordered tug-of-war. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 8430–8434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rege, M., & Telle, K. (2004). The impact of social approval and framing on cooperation in public good situations. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1625–1644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigdon, M., Ishii, K., Watabe, M., & Kitayama, S. (2009). Minimal social cues in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 358–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G. (1998). Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 265, 427–431.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, G. (2005). Cooperation through interdependence. Animal Behaviour, 70, 901–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. (2004). The effect of social role on theory of mind reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 91–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schelling, T. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate-proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 38–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semmann, D., Krambeck, H.-J., & Milinski, M. (2004). Strategic investment in reputation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56, 248–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2004). Patterns of coalition formation by adult female baboons in Amboseli, Kenya. Animal Behaviour, 67, 573–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature, 15, 343–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., & Bird, R. B. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening: Public generosity as costly signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 245–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., Bird, R. B., & Bird, D. W. (2003). The benefits of costly signalling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behavioural Ecology, 14, 116–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, S. E. (1985). Women’s intuition: The effect of subordinate role on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 146–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, S. E. (1992). Further effects of role versus gender on interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 154–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparks, A., & Barclay, P. (2013). Eyes increase generosity, but not for long: The limited effect of a false cue. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 317–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sylwester, K., & Roberts, G. (2010). Cooperators benefit through reputation-based partner choice in economic games. Biology Letters, 6, 659–662.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. L., Franks, D. D., & Calonico, J. M. (1972). Role-taking and power in social psychology. American Sociological Review, 37, 605–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topline Strategy Group. (2007). Study challenges idea of hybrid auto buyers as typical early adopters. http://www.toplinestrategy.com/pr_4_23_07.htm.

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 119–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toyota Reports 2007 and December Sales. (2008). http://www.toyota.com/about/news/corporate/2008/01/03-1-sales.html.

  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Soest, D. P., & Vyrastekova, J. (2004). Economic ties and social dilemmas: An economic experiment. CentER Discussion Paper 2004-55, CentER, University of Tilburg, Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M. (2006). Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership. Personality and Psychology Review, 10, 354–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M., & Hardy, C. L. (2010). Cooperation for reputation: Wasteful contributions as costly signals in public goods. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13, 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wedekind, C., & Milinski, M. (2000). Cooperation through image scoring in humans. Science, 288, 850–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willer, R. (2009). Groups reward individual sacrifice: The status solution to the collective action problem. American Sociological Review, 74, 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 110–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pat Barclay .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kafashan, S., Sparks, A., Griskevicius, V., Barclay, P. (2014). Prosocial Behavior and Social Status. In: Cheng, J., Tracy, J., Anderson, C. (eds) The Psychology of Social Status. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0867-7_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics