Skip to main content

Hunter-Gatherers: Problems in Theory

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Hunter-Gatherers

Abstract

In this concluding chapter, we discuss the important difference between theories that focus on consequences and ones that focus on processes. Most theories in anthropology rest on generalizations about consequences and, as such, cannot be reconciled with the natural sciences or a Darwinian approach to hunter-gatherers. The neo-Darwinian theories presented in the previous chapters are theories of processes that allow for a truly evolutionary approach to cultural change in which ideas and the motives of individuals—the most distinctive part of the thing we call culture—have an active part and in which selection can play a definable role. Human behavioral ecology and cultural transmission theory differ primarily in the locus and mode of operation of the opposing forces of selection and reproduction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Sex ratios are a case in point. Simple adaptive logic suggests that efficient sexually reproducing populations should have many fewer males than females (only enough to fertilize all females) yet in most sexually reproducing species there are as many males as females. A simple model explains why this is so (Fisher 1958). Suppose sex of offspring is genetically controlled (which it is). Suppose further that for some reason in a population there are many more females than males. This situation favors individuals who tend to produce males because when males are rarer they individually have more opportunities to mate than more numerous females. This advantage for male-producing genotypes persists until the sex ratio becomes even. For the same reason, when there are more males than females, female-producing genotypes will be favored.

References

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1980). Explanatory/predictive models of hunter-gatherer adaptation. In M.B. Schiffer (Ed.), Advances in archaeological method and theory, Vol. 3 (pp. 189–255). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1962). Archaeology as anthropology. American Antiquity, 28, 217–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1968a). Archaeological perspectives. In S. R. Binford & L. R. Binford (Eds.), New perspectives in archaeology (pp. 5–32). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1968b). Post-Pleistocene adaptations. In S. R. Binford & L. R. Binford (Eds.), New perspectives in archaeology (pp. 313–341). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1977). General introduction. In L. R. Binford (Ed.), For theory building in archaeology (pp. 1–10). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1983). In pursuit of the past: Decoding the archaeological record. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burrow, J. W. (1966). Evolution and society: A study in Victorian social theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. (1980). Evolutionary theory and archaeology. In M.B. Schiffer (Ed.), Advances in archaeological method and theory, Vol. 3 (pp. 35–99). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L., Eysenck, H., & Martin, N. (1989). Genes, culture and personality. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. (1958). The genetical theory of natural selection. (Revised ed.). New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flannery, K. V. (1968). Archaeological system theory and early Mesoamerica. In B. Meggers (Ed.), Anthropological archaeology in the Americas (pp. 67–87). Washington, DC: Anthropological Society of Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flannery, K. V. (1972). The cultural evolution of civilizations. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 3, 399–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foin, T. C., & Davis, W. G. (1984). Ritual and self-regulation of the Tsembaga Maring ecosystem in the New Guinea highlands. Human Ecology, 12, 385–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. A. (1954). On the concept of types. American Anthropologist, 56, 42–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 205, 581–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallpike, C. R. (1986). The principles of social evolution. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1968). The rise of anthropological theory. New York: Crowell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1974). Cows, pigs, wars, and witches: The riddles of culture. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1977). Cannibals and kings: The origins of cultures. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1979). Cultural materialism. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peel, J. D. Y. (1972). Herbert Spencer on social evolution: Selected writings. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R. A. (1968). Pigs for the ancestors. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, J. R. (1986). Style, function, and assemblage variability: A reply to Binford. American Antiquity, 51, 628–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1868). Progress: Its law and cause. Essays I: 1–60. London: Williams and Norgate (Original Westminster Review, April 1857).

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1898). Principles of biology I. (Revised and enlarged edition). New York: Appleton. (Original 1866).

    Google Scholar 

  • Steward, J. H. (1938). Basin-Plateau aboriginal sociopolitical groups. Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin, 120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, W. W. (1948). A study of archaeology. American Anthropological Association Memoir, 69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, W. W. (1972). Old wine and new skins. In M. Leone (Ed.), Contemporary archaeology: A guide to theory and contributions (pp. 28–33). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vayda, A. P., & Rappaport, R. A. (1968). Ecology, cultural and non-cultural. In J. A. Clifton (Ed.), Introduction to cultural anthropology (pp. 477–497). Boston: Houghton.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, L. (1959). The evolution of culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert L. Bettinger .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bettinger, R., Garvey, R., Tushingham, S. (2015). Hunter-Gatherers: Problems in Theory. In: Hunter-Gatherers. Interdisciplinary Contributions to Archaeology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-7581-2_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics