Skip to main content

Some Aspects of the Embryology of Behavior

  • Chapter
Neuroembryology
  • 160 Accesses

Abstract

In the past, the development of behavior in vertebrate embryos has been interpreted in terms of two opposing viewpoints. Coghill, on the basis of his observations on the salamander Ambystoma, has maintained that behavior represents an integrated pattern from beginning to end; partial patterns segregate from total patterns by a developmental process which he calls “individuation.” The opposing view, advocated by Windle and other students of the behavior of mammalian embryos and fetuses, contends that local reflexes are the elementary units of behavior that combine secondarily to form integrated patterns

The conceptual and factual foundations of both theories are reappraised, and their limitations and shortcomings are indicated. It becomes evident that Coghill’s concepts, derived from lower forms, cannot be generalized and extended to higher forms. On the other hand, the reflexogenic theory is unacceptable on theoretical and factual grounds.

A reinvestigation of the embryology of behavior in the chick embryo has provided a new frame of reference that places the problem in a different perspective. An old observation of Preyer (1885) to the effect that overt motility is in operation several days before reflex circuits are formed has been followed up, and, in a series of observations and experiments, spontaneous, nonreflexogenic motility was established as a basic component of embryonic behavior. It can be shown that spontaneous motility patterns are not influenced by sensory input. Arguments are presented in favor of the idea that the spontaneous motility in the chick embryo is due to automatic, self-generated activity of neurons rather than to stimulation by agents in the circulation. One of the most significant characteristics of spontaneous motility is its rhythmicity: short phases of activity are followed by longer phases of inactivity. The analysis of recordings of the periodicity in normal embryos and in embryos with transected spinal cords has enabled us to dissociate a trigger mechanism for activation of motility from a device that determines the duration of the activity phases.

Spontaneous and reflexogenic motility are regarded as two independent basic constituents of embryonic behavior. The former is considered to be the primary component, not only because it precedes the reflex responses in lower forms but mainly because reflex motility remains latent in the embryo owing to the absence of adequate stimuli; therefore, it cannot contribute to the molding of behavior patterns in prehatching and prenatal stages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

List of Literature

  • Adrian, E. D. 1931. Potential changes in the isolated nervous system of Dytiscus marginalis. J. Physiol., 72: 132–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angulo, A. W. 1932. The prenatal development of behavior in the albino rat. J. Comp. Neurol., 55: 395–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barcroft, J., and D. H. Barron. 1939. The development of behavior in foetal sheep. J. Comp. Neurol., 70: 477–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, D. H. 1941. The functional development of some mammalian neuromuscular mechanisms. Biol. Rev., 16: 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, D. H. 1950. Genetic neurology and the behavior problem. In Genetic Neurology, P. Weiss, (ed.), pp. 223–231. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bridgman, C. S., and L. Carmichael. 1935. An experimental study of the onset of behavior in the fetal guinea pig. J. Genet. Psychol., 47: 247–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinley, F. S. 1951. Effects of curare on fish embryos. Physiol. Zool., 24: 186–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, T. H. 1961. The origins of patterned nervous discharge. Behaviour, 17. 48–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, T. H. 1962. Integration and rhythmicity in neural systems. Am. Zool. 2: 97–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmichael, L. 1934. An experimental study in the prenatal guinea pig of the origin and development of reflexes and patterns of behavior in relation to the stimulation of specific receptor areas during the period of active fetal life. Genet. Psychol. Monogr., 16: 337–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmichael, L. 1954. The onset and early development of behavior. In Manual of Child Psychology, L. Carmichael, (ed.), pp. 60–185. John Wiley & Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. 1929a. Anatomy and the Problem of Behaviour. 113 pp. Cambridge, at the University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. 1929b. The early development of behavior in Amblystoma and in man. Arch. Neurol. Psychol., 21: 989–1009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. 1933. Somatic myogenic action in embryos of Fundulus heteroclitus. Proc. Soc. Exptl. Biol. Med., 31: 62–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. 1940. Early embryonic somatic movements in birds and in mammals other than man. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop., Natl. Res. Council, Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S. M., and E. R. Peterson. 1963. Bioelectrical activity in long-term cultures of spinal cord tissues. Science, 141: 427–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. W. B. 1962. Qualitative behavior of spontaneous potentials from expiants of 15-day chick embryo telencephalon in vitro. J. Gen. Physiol, 45: 1065–1076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. W. B., and B. J. Rylander. 1961. Behavior of spontaneous potentials from chick cerebellar expiants during 120 hours in culture. J. Neuro-physiol, 24: 141–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drachman, D. B., and A. C. Coulombre. 1962. Experimental clubfoot and arthrogryposis multiplex congenita. Lancet, Sept. 15, 523–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flexner, L. B. 1955. Enzymatic and functional patterns of the developing mammalian brain. In Biochemistry of the Developing Nervous System, H. Waelsch, (ed.), pp. 281–300. Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V. 1960. A Manual of Experimental Embryology, 221 pp. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V. 1964. Ontogeny of behavior and its structural basis. In Comparative Neurochemistry, D. Richter (ed.), p. 21–34. Pergamon Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V., and M. Balaban. 1963. Observations and experiments on spontaneous rhythmical behavior in the chick embryo. Develop. Biol., 7: 533–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V., and H. L. Hamilton. 1951. A series of normal stages in the development of the chick embryo. J. Morphol., 88: 49–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V., and M. Waugh. 1940. The primary development of the skeleton in nerveless and poorly innervated limb transplants of chick embryos. Physiol. Zool., 13: 367–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. E., and H. P. Whiting. 1954. Structure and function in the locomotory system of the dogfish embryo. The myogenic state of movement. J. Exptl. Biol., 31: 501–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrick, C. J. 1949. George Ellett Coghill, Naturalist and Philosopher, 280 pp. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtzer, H., and R. P. Kamrin. 1956. Development of local coordination centers. I. Brachial centers in the salamander spinal cord. J. Exptl. Zool., 132: 391–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hooker, D. 1952. The Prenatal Origin of Behavior, 143 pp. University of Kansas Press, Lawrence.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, T. 1952. The spinal tract of the trigeminal nerve in human embryos between 71/2 and 81/2 weeks of menstrual age and its relation to early fetal behavior. J. Comp. Neurol., 97: 143–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z. Y. 1932. Ontogeny of embryonic behavior in Aves. I. The chronology and general nature of the behavior of the chick embryo. J. Exptl. Zool., 61: 395–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z. Y. 1939a. Studies in the physiology of the embryonic nervous system. I. Effect of curare on motor activity of the chick embryo. J. Exptl. Zool., 82: 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z. Y. 1939b. Studies in the physiology of the embryonic nervous system. II. Experimental evidence on the controversy over the reflex theory in development. J. Comp. Neurol., 70: 437–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z. Y. 1939c. Total pattern or local reflexes? Psychol. Rev., 46: 93–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orr, D. W., and W. F. Windle. 1934. The development of behavior in chick embryos: The appearance of somatic movements. J. Comp. Neurol., 60: 271–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J. J., A. R. Vonderahe, and A. A. Huesman. 1960. Chronological development of electrical activity in the optic lobes, cerebellum and cerebrum of the chick embryo. Physiol. Zool., 33: 225–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preyer, W. 1885. Specielle Physiologie des Embryo, 644 pp. Grieben, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, H. C. 1926. The development of motility and behavior in the toadfish (Opsanus tau). J. Comp. Neurol., 40: 253–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visintini, F., and R. Levi-Montalcini. 1939. Relazione tra differenziazione strutturale e fun-zionale dei centri e délie vie nervöse nell’embrione di polio. Arch. Suisses Neurol. Psychiat., 43: 1–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. 1941a. Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of coordination. Comp. Psychol. Monogr., 17: 1–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. 1941b. Autonomous versus reflexogenous activity of the central nervous system. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc, 84: 53–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, H. P. 1955. Functional development in the nervous system. In Biochemistry of the Developing Nervous System, H. Waelsch (ed.), pp. 85–103. Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windle, W. F. 1940. Physiology of the Fetus, 249 pp. W. B. Saunders, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windle, W. F. 1944. Genesis of somatic motor function in mammalian embryos: a synthesizing article. Physiol. Zool., 17: 247–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windle, W. F., and A. M. Griffin. 1931. Observations on embryonic and fetal movements of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol, 52: 149–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, W. (ed.). 1962. Rhythmic functions in the living system. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 98: 753-1326.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Dedicated to my friend, Professor F. Baltzer, Berne, Switzerland, on the occasion of his 80th birthday

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Springer Basel AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hamburger, V. (1990). Some Aspects of the Embryology of Behavior. In: Neuroembryology. Birkhäuser, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-6743-5_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-6743-5_9

  • Publisher Name: Birkhäuser, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-6745-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-6743-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics