Skip to main content

Neuropsychiatry’s Current State

  • Chapter
  • 46 Accesses

Abstract

In recent years, an avalanche of data, comprising more than 95 percent of all the brain findings ever linked to behavior, has inundated psychiatry.1 New investigative tools have uncovered a vast storehouse of measurable brain oddities that correlate seductively with classical psychiatric syndromes. In particular, state-of-the-art computerized scanners2–4 now provide increasingly fine resolution of brain images that have led to a bonanza of apparent empirical support for the neuropsychiatric school of thought.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. Yudofsky and Hales, “The Reemergence of Neuropsychiatry,” p. 4.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 108, 172–173, 176.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Nancy Andreasen, Gregg Cohen, Greg Harris, Ted Cizaldo, Jussi Parkkinen, Karim Rezai, and Victor Swayze, “Image Processing for the Study of Brain Structure and Function: Problems and Programs,” Journal of Neuropsychiatry 4:2 (Spring, 1992), pp. 125–133.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Robert Hendren and Janet Hodde-Vargas, “Neuroimaging in Schizophrenia,” Psychiatric Times (September 1992), pp. 17–18.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Jeffrey Coffman, “Computed Tomography in Psychiatry,” in Brain Imaging: Applications in Psychiatry, ed. Nancy Andreasen (Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, 1989), pp. 1–3.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Nancy Andreasen, “Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Imaging,” in Andreasen, ed., Brain Imaging, p. 67.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Edward Edelson, “Scanning the Body Magnetic,” Science 83 (July-August 1983), pp. 60–65.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Michael Devous, “Imaging Brain Function by Single-Photon Emission Computer Tomograph,” in Andreasen, ed., Brain Imaging, pp. 148–150.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Henry Holcomb, Jonathan Links, Caroline Smith, and Dean Wong, “Positron Emission Tomography: Measuring the Metabolic and Neurochemical Characteristics of the Living Human Nervous System,” in Andreasen, ed., Brain Imaging, pp. 235–236.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Sandra Blakeslee, “Scanner Pinpoints Site of Thought as Brain Sees or Speaks,” New York Times (1 June 1993), pp. Cl, C3.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Gina Kolata, “Improved Scanner Watches the Brain As It Thinks,” New York Times (14 July 1992), pp. Cl, C6.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Nora D. Volkow and Laurence R. Tancredi, “Biological Correlates of Mental Activity Studied With PET,” American Journal of Psychiatry USA (April 1991), pp. 439–440, 442.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 166–171, 226.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Hendren and Hodde-Vargas, pp. 19–20.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Tomb, Psychiatry for the House Officer, p. 25.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, p. 178.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Andreasen et al., “Image Processing,” pp. 125–133.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Ruben Gur, Roland Erwin, and Raquel Gur, “Neurobehavioral Probes, for Physiologic Neuroimaging Studies,” Archives of General Psychiatry 49 (May 1992), pp. 409–414.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Hendren and Hodde-Vargas, pp. 18–20.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Robert Innis, “Neuroreceptor Imaging with SPECT,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:11 (November 1992 supplement), pp. 29–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. John Lauerman, “Windows on the Brain: Functional Brain Imaging Techniques Add Objective Scale to Psychiatric Evaluation and Treatment,” Psychiatric Times (February 1993), pp. 16–17.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Donald Mender, notes for lecture on CT, MRI and PET scanning in psychiatry given in 1988 at St. John’s Episcopal Hospital, Smithtown, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Marc Schuckit, “An Introduction and Overview to Clinical Applications of Neuro SPECT in Psychiatry,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:11 (November 1992 supplement), pp. 3–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ronald Van Heertum, “Brain SPECT Imaging and Psychiatry,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:11 (November 1992 supplement), pp. 7–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Nora Volkow and Laurence Tancredi, “Current and Future Applications of SPECT in Clinical Psychiatry,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:11 (November 1992 supplement), pp. 26–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Scott Woods, “Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Imaging with SPECT in Psychiatric Disease: Focus on Schizophrenia, Anxiety Disorders, and Substance Abuse,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:11 (November 1992 supplement), pp. 20–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Blakeslee, “Scanner,” pp. C-1, C-3.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Kolata, pp. C1, C6.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Volkow and Tancredi, “Biological Correlates,” pp. 439–440, 442.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 28, 120–121, 194, 225, 238.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Juan M. De Lecuona, K. Sunny Joseph, Naveed Iqbal, and Gregory Asis, “Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia Revisited,” Psychiatric Annals 23:4 (April 1993), pp. 179–185.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Tomb, pp. 25–26.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Winson, p. 238.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 233–234.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Tomb, pp. 42–43, 45–46.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 181–182, 234.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Burr Eichelman, “Aggressive Behavior: From Laboratory to Clinic,” Archives of General Psychiatry 49 (June 1992), pp. 489–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Markku Linnoila and Matti Virkkunen, “Aggression, Suicidally, and Serotonin,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:10 (October 1992 supplement), pp. 46–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, p. 230.

    Google Scholar 

  40. “Genes, Viruses, Hormones and Schizophrenia Etiology,” Clinical Psychiatry News, August 1992, p. 22.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 185–188.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Morton Low, “Evaluation of Psychiatric Disorders and the Effects of Psychotherapeutic and Psychotomimetic Agents,” in Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography, ed. Donald Klass and David Daly (New York: Raven, 1979), pp. 395–410.

    Google Scholar 

  43. John Horgan, “Eugenics Revisited,” Scientific American 268 (June 1993), pp. 122–131.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Kaplan and Sadock, Synopsis of Psychiatry, p. 79.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Larry J. Siever and Kenneth L. Davis, “A Psychobiological Perspective on the Personality Disorders,” American Journal of Psychiatry 148:12 (December 1991), p. 1648.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Tomb, pp. 41–43, 153–155.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Eichelman, p. 489.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Lishman, pp. 168–171.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Arnold Merriam, Alice A. Medalia, and Bernard Wyszynski, “Schizophrenia as a Neurobehavioral Disorder,” Psychiatric Annals 23:4 (April 1993), pp. 171–174.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Richard Restak, “See No Evil,” The Sciences (July-August 1992), p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Tardiff, p. 495.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Tomb, pp. 123–124.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Since the 1970s, a syndrome first labeled “episodic dyscontrol” and now known as “intermittent explosive disorder” began to draw particular psychiatric interest in the quest to explain human violence as a limbic disturbance. Patients with this diagnosis are prone to sudden assaultive episodes; between paroxysms their behavior remains nonviolent, and they may experience remorse or amnesia for their destructive acts. Affected patients are more likely than the general population to have undergone traumatic births, exhibited behavioral difficulties as children, caused motor vehicle accidents, and shown constitutional sensitivities to alcohol. On laboratory testing, many demonstrate electrical disturbances and other evidence of limbic brain damage. Sparser data from different patients implicate damage within nonlimbic regions, such as frontal structures that regulate social inhibitions, in the genesis of aggressive behavior. Evidence includes a generally increased but still rough association of violence with seizures, head injuries, abuse during childhood, malnutrition, learning disabilities, diminished IQ, subtle defects on neuropsychological testing, and assorted electroencephalographic abnormalities.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 120–121.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Eichelman, p. 489.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Merriam et ah, pp. 174–177.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Restak, “See No Evil,” pp. 18, 19.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Tardiff, pp. 494–495.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Tomb, pp. 122–123.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Martin Drooker and Robert Byck, “Physical Disorders Presenting as Psychiatric Illness: A New View,” Psychiatric Times (July 1992), pp. 19–23.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Tomb, pp. 118–120.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Frank Ayd, “Novel Antipsychotics, Antidepressants Surveyed in S. E,” Psychiatric Times (August 1993), pp. 7–8.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Miriam Tucker, “Clozapine-like Antipsychotics Seen as Wave of the Future,” Clinical Psychiatric News (January 1993), pp. 1,13.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Freedman, p. 862.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Jonathan Pincus and Gary Tucker, Behavioral Neurology (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1985), pp. 131, 137–138.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 222, 230.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Winson, p. 238.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, pp. 136–137.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Linda Barr, “Serotonin in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder,” Psychiatric Times (July 1993), pp. 24–25.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Jeffrey Cummings, “Neurobiological Basis of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder,” Psychiatric Times (January 1993), p. 16.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Roberto Dominguez, “Serotonergic Antidepressants and Their Efficacy in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53:10 (October 1992 supplement), pp. 56–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Tomb, pp. 75, 79.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Siever and Davis, pp. 1648–1650, 1653.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Schizotypal personality disorders and frank schizophrenia in particular share common genetic determinants, atten-tional deficits, eye movement abnormalities, and, during crises, increased dopamine metabolism. Among impulsive personality disorders, certain varieties tend to occur in the same families, suggesting a genetic element common to such subtypes. Impulsively antisocial traits correlate with biological indices of an impaired ability to suppress negatively reinforced behaviors; the indices include a reduced sedation threshold, disinhibited motor function, accelerated habituation of skin conductance, and decreased sympathetic response. Patients whose personality disturbances involve mood lability exhibit sleep patterns similar to those seen in depression, exaggerated behavioral reactions to drugs that enhance the action of norepinephrine, and improvement after treatment with several other chemicals that affect neurotransmitter function. Personality disorders dominated by anxiety show chronically reduced thresholds for sedation, weak habituation, and sympathetic overactivity (Siever and Davis, pp. 1648–1655).

    Google Scholar 

  75. Siever and Davis, pp. 1651–1653.

    Google Scholar 

  76. De Lecuona et al, pp. 182, 184.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Kaplan and Sadock, pp. 72–74.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Ross Baldessarini, Chemotherapy in Psychiatry (Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1985), p. 40.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Tomb, pp. 25–26.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Paul Genova, “Is American Psychiatry Terminally Ill?” Psychiatric Times (June 1993), p. 19.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Tomb, pp. 26–27.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Horgan, p. 127.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Capra, p. 120.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Unfortunately, the molecular paradigm, applied successfully to the structure of the gene in the 1940s, 1950s, and 1960s, has today become overgeneralized in its application to other areas, including neuropsychiatric research (Capra, pp. 103–104, 113–114, 119–120).

    Google Scholar 

  85. Andreasen, The Broken Brain, p. 182–183.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Kaplan and Sadock, p. 74.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Tomb, pp. 42–43, 45–46.

    Google Scholar 

  88. It is a gross oversimplification to seek only single causes for any illness. The “single lesion” myth of disease causality arose originally in the germ theories of the nineteenth century, expanded into molecular biology and genetics, and has now been misapplied to the treatment of many multifactorial disease processes. A more balanced view must also account for the fact that the response of a host organism contributes at least as much to pathological states as does the virulence of a noxious agent (Capra, pp. 111, 150, 153–154).

    Google Scholar 

  89. Perceptual distortions only indirectly related to brain processes seem to figure strongly in the genesis of aggression. Schizophrenia, mania, cocaine-induced paranoia, and other delusional states number among the most potent identified antecedents of violence. Beyond frank psychosis, simple mismatch between perception and behavioral abilities— producing, for example, frustration in the mentally retarded— can lead to assaultive outbursts. Animal models support these observations in humans: External interference in rats’ ability to smell alters aggressive behavior in experimental contexts. Social factors correlate with violent behavior. Animal studies suggest that some brain disease processes produce violence only indirectly by changing territorial behavior. The politics of social dominance and even parental input contribute to violence in societies of rodents and primates. Among human psychiatric patients, such factors as institutional settings, interpersonal space, childhood neglect, inappropriate physical restraint, domestic trigger situations, and the psychotherapist’s behavior may all play inflammatory roles. The most powerful predictors of violence in clinical psychiatric practice turn out to be such quasi-tautological factors as previous assaultive history, violent plans, availability of a weapon, and past record of other impulsive behaviors like suicide, sexual promiscuity, profligate spending, property destruction, and reckless driving.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Eichelman, pp. 489–490.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Restak, “See No Evil,” pp. 18–19.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Tardiff, pp. 494–498.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Freedman, pp. 858–859.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Theodore Lidz, “Images in Psychiatry: Adolf Meyer,” American Journal of Psychiatry 150:7 (July 1993), p. 1098.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Owen Flanagan, The Science of Mind (Cambridge: M.I.T. Press, 1991), p. 95.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Steven Cooper, “Neuroscience in the Future of Psychiatry,” American Journal of Psychiatry 145:2 (February 1988), p. 272.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  97. W. W. Meissner, “The Psychotherapies: Individual, Family, and Group,” in Nicholi, ed., The New Harvard Guide, p. 463.

    Google Scholar 

  98. Tomb, pp. 154–155.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Eric Marcus, Psychosis and Near Psychosis (New York: Springer-Verlag, 1992), pp. 217–249.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  100. Glen Gabbard, Susan Lazar, and Elizabeth Hersh, “Cost-Offset Studies Show Value of Psychotherapy,” Psychiatric Times (August 1993), p. 21.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Yudofsky and Hales, “The Reemergence,” p. 4.

    Google Scholar 

  102. “Don’t Replace Psychotherapy With a Pill, Experts Remind Public-Sector Psychiatrists,” Psychiatric News (3 July 1992), p. 13.

    Google Scholar 

  103. Elio Frattaroli, “The Mind-Body Problem and the Choice of Intervention,” The Psychiatric Times (November 1991), pp. 75–76.

    Google Scholar 

  104. Gabbard et al., p. 21.

    Google Scholar 

  105. Restak, “See No Evil,” p. 20.

    Google Scholar 

  106. American Psychiatric Association, Biographical Directory (Washington: American Psychiatric Association, 1989), p. 1573.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Thomas Szasz, The Myth of Mental Illness (New York: Harper & Row, 1974), p. 262.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Frattaroli, p. 73.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Genova, p. 20.

    Google Scholar 

  110. An extreme example of neuropsychiatric control of patients by doctors occurred in Russia. This was not simply a result of Communist totalitarianism. Balinksy first imported the neuropsychiatric tradition from Greisinger’s Germany to St. Petersburg, home of the Russian Tsar, in the nineteenth century. Korsakov, Bekhterev, Sechenov, and Pavlov then promoted neuropsychiatry in pre-Revolutionary Russia. Its hold hardened into stone when the first Russian institute of psychology opened at the University of Moscow in 1911; the institute purposely imported German staff and materialist ideas to use as its own. Though the chaos of the Soviet Revolution in 1917 brought a brief respite of ferment and eclectism, the grip of materialistic dogma soon afterward grew tighter than ever and finally became a stranglehold around psychiatric practice. From then on, classification of psychiatric illnesses in Russia embraced an almost exclusively somatic bias. Its criteria for psychiatric diagnoses included electroencephalographic findings, odd body shapes, and even heart abnormalities. Punitive neuropsychiatric treatments like sulfazine, atropine, and insulin coma were employed (Mender, notes for a seminar on Soviet psychiatric diagnosis).

    Google Scholar 

  111. Capra, pp. 134, 148–149.

    Google Scholar 

  112. The early success of chemicals in preventing and curing infectious diseases inspires modern enthusiasm for drug treatment techniques in general; however, confusion abounds regarding the real cause of this success. The decrease in infectious diseases over the last 150 years has stemmed more from general improvement in nutrition and sanitary conditions than from any other factor (Capra, pp. 131–142).

    Google Scholar 

  113. Richard Restak, “Brain by Design,” The Sciences (September-October, 1993), pp. 32–33.

    Google Scholar 

  114. Capra, pp. 155, 249.

    Google Scholar 

  115. Restak, “Brain by Design,” pp. 32–33.

    Google Scholar 

  116. Herbert Marcuse, One-Dimensional Man (Boston: Beacon Press, 1964), p. 158.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Hearnshaw, p. 122.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1994 Donald Mender

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mender, D. (1994). Neuropsychiatry’s Current State. In: The Myth of Neuropsychiatry. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-6010-8_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-6010-8_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-306-44652-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-6010-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics