Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Perspectives on Individual Differences ((PIDF))

Abstract

Perceptual science occupies a rather interesting position within the cognate areas of neuropsychology. It is perhaps the only discipline that is, at once, both widely applied in neuropsychology and yet seems to have had no major impact in the way of producing a transfer of constructs and influencing the conceptual basis of neuropsychological assessment. Consider how current constructs from memory research such as “depth of processing” or “episodic memory” have worked their way into neuropsychological analysis. Parallels from perception are hard to find. Given the long history of research and theory construction in perception and, indeed, its central position in the history of psychology, one would expect a tremendously active and very current use of perceptual constructs in neuropsychology. Perhaps one source of perceptual science’s lack of vigorous input into neuropsychology is the immediacy of the content, the apparent face validity of any construct that one uses, at any level of analysis. Consider the example of “visual closure.” People do tend to fill in open forms as the Gestalt law of closure aptly demonstrates. It is not surprising, therefore, that Colarusso and Hammill (1972) examine this “basic” skill as one of five types in their Motor-Free Visual Perception Test. Here the child must choose the correct (closed) form from various figures drawn incompletely with gaps within one or more line elements.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Acredolo, L. P. The familiarity factor in spatial research. In R. Cohen (Ed.), New directions for child development: Children’s conceptions of spatial relationships. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P., & Hake, J. L. Infant perception. In B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L. S., & Williams, T. M. Development of multiple dimension use in form classification. Child Development, 1975, 46, 123–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Appelle, S. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: The oblique effect in man and animals. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 78, 266–278.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aschkenasy, J. R., & Odom, K. D. Classification and perceptual development: Exploring issues about integrality and differential sensitivity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1982, 34, 435–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N. Development of binocular fixation in human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977, 23, 133–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F. Some informational aspects of visual perception. Psychological Review, 1954, 61, 183–193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F. Symmetry, information, and memory for patterns. American Journal of Psychology, 1955, 68, 209–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F. Physical determinants of the judged complexity of shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1957, 53, 221–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, M. S. The development of visual accommodation during early infancy. Child Development, 1980, 51, 646–666.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, J. Effect of orientation and of shape similarity on perceptual grouping. Perception & Psychophysics, 1966, 1, 300–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belmont, I. Perceptual organization and minimal brain dysfunctions. In H. E. Rie & E. D. Rie (Eds.), Handbook of minimal brain dysfunction: A critical view. New York: Wiley, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, E. D., & Ehrfurth, J. W. The continued inappropriate singular use of the Bender Visual Motor Gestalt Test. Professional Psychology, 1981, 12, 562–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boder, E. Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical readingspelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1973, 15, 663–687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Ferdinandsen, K., & Gross, C. G. Perception of symmetry in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 1981, 17, 82–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. R., & Owens, D. H. The metrics of visual form: Methodological dispepsin. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68, 243–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chipman, S. F. & Mendelson, M. J. The development of sensitivity to visual structure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1975, 20, 411–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chipman, S. F., & Mendelson, M. J. Influence of six types of visual structure on complexity judgments in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1979, 5, 365–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. The role of activity in the construction of spatial representations. In R. Cohen (Ed.), New directions for child development: Children’s conception of spatial relationships. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colarusso, R. P., & Hammill, D. D. Motor-free visual perception test. Novato, Calif.: Academic Therapy, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. & Zalik, M. C. Why do children confuse mirror-image obliques? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977, 24, 516–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durant, W. The story of philosophy. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Essock, E. A. The oblique effect of stimulus identification considered with respect to two classes of oblique effects. Perception, 1980, 9, 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantz, R. L. Pattern vision in young infants. Psychological Record, 1958, 8, 43–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fantz, R. L. Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science, 1963, 140, 296–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fantz, R. L., & Miranda, S. B. Newborn infant attention to form of contour. Child Development, 1975, 46, 224–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C. B., Ferdinandsen, K., & Bornstein, M. H. The role of symmetry in infant form discrimination. Child Development, 1981, 52, 457–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M. Linguistic factors in reading acquisition: Evidence for developmental changes. In F. J. Pirozzolo & M. C. Wittrock (Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading. New York: Academic Press, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., & Satz, P. Developmental changes in the neuropsychological correlates of reading achievement: A six-year longitudinal follow-up. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1980, 2, 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgus, R. H., & Melamed, L. E. Perception: A cognitive-stage approach. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frostig, M. Developmental test of visual perception. Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. R. The processing of information and structure. Potomac, Md.: Erlbaum, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geyer, L. H. A two-channel theory of short-term visual storage. (Doctoral dissertation, SUNY at Buffalo, Buffalo, N.Y., 1970). (University Microfilms, No. 71-7165).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. Principles of perceptual learning and development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Levin, H. The psychology of reading. Cambridge: The M.I.T. Press, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Owsley, C. J., & Johnston, J. Perception of invariants by five-month-old infants: Differentiation of two types of motion. Developmental Psychology, 1978, 14, 407–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Owsley, C. J., Walker, A., & Megaw-Nyce, J. Development of the perception of invariants: Substance and shape. Perception, 1979, 8, 609–619.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. The “visual cliff.” Scientific American, 1960, 202, 64–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, C. J. The Luria-Nebraska children’s battery: Theory and formulation. In G. W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the school-age child. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., Le Tendre, J. B., & Bishop, A. The young child’s discrimination of obliques. Perception, 1974, 3, 261–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartlage, L. C. & Telzrow, C. F. The neuropsychological basis of educational intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilties, 1983, 16, 521–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth, F. Mediating the so-called immediate processes of perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 386–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helmholtz, H. Treatise on physiological optics. J. P. S. Southall (Ed.), translated from the 3rd German edition. New York: Optical Society of America, 1924. (Reprinted by Dover, 1962.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg, J. Sensation and perception. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillside, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg, J. Levels of perceptual organization. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization. Hillside, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. K., & Pick, A. D. On the nature of information in behalf of direct perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 388–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kavale, K., & Mattson, D. One jumped off the balance beam: Meta-analysis of perceptualmotor training. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1983, 16, 165–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kemler, D. G., & Smith, L. B. Accessing similarity and dimensional relations: Effects of integrality and separability on the discovery of complex concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1979, 108, 133–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. Principles of gestalt psychology. New York, Harcourt Brace, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. The growth of the mind. New York: The Humanities Press, 1951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., Pick, H. L., & Fanello, G. R. Cognitive maps in children and men. Child Development, 1974, 45, 707–716.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuennapas, T., & Janson, A. J. Multidimensional similarity of letters. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1969, 28, 3–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., Gleitman, H., & Spelke, E. Spatial knowledge and geometric representation in a child blind from birth. Science, 1981, 213, 1275–1277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, S. C., & Hammill, D. D. The relationship of selected visual-perceptual abilities to school learning. Journal of Special Education, 1975, 9, 281–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughery, K. R. Computer simulation of short-term memory: A component decay model. In G. T. Bower & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. VI). New York: Academic Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, R., Stewart, N., & Freedman, D. Neuropsychological characteristics of empirically derived subgroups of learning disabled readers. Journal of Clinical Neruopsychology, 1982, 4, 343–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maisel, E. B., & Karmel, B. Z. Contour density and pattern configuration in visual perferences of infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 1978, 1, 127–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D., & Nishihara, H. K. Visual information processing: Artificial intelligence and the sensorium of sight. Technology Beview, 1978, 81, 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattis, S., French, J. H., & Rapin, I. Dyslexia in children and young adults: Three independent neuropsychological syndromes. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1975, 17, 150–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miccinati, J. Teach reading disabled students to perceive distinctive features in words. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1981, 14, 140–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pastore, N. Selective history of theories of visual perception: 1630–1950. New York: Oxford University Press, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pick, H. L., & Rieser, J. J. Children’s cognitive mapping. In M. Potegal (Ed.), Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations. New York: Academic Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R. Perceptual organization in information processing. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual Organization. Hilldale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R., & Kubovy, M. Perceptual organization: An overview. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization. Hilldale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S., & Jones, R. K. James Gibsons ecological revolution in psychology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 1979, 9, 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. An introduction to perception. New York: Macmillan, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I. Difficulties with a direct theory of perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 398–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rock, I., Hill, A. L., & Fineman, M. Speed constancy as a function of size constancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 1968, 4, 37–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B. P. Central processing deficiencies in children: Toward a developmental neuropsychological model. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1982, 4, 1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B. P., Bakker, D. J., Fisk, J. L., & Strang, J. D. Child neuropsychology: An introduction to theory, research, and clinical practice. New York: Guilford Press, 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudel, R. G. The oblique mystique: A slant on the development of spatial coordinates. In M. Potegal (Ed.), Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations. New York: Academic Press, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A. The development of perception and recognition of objects. Child Development, 1980, 51, 981–992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A. Effect of object movement on infants’ detection of object structure. Developmental Psychology, 1982, 18, 462–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rugle, L., & Melamed, L. E. Perceptual processing abilities and school achievement in young children: Longitudinal findings with a construct valid perceptual processing instrument. Submitted for publication, 1985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., & Morris, R. Learning disability subtypes: A review. In F. J. Pirozzolo & M. C. Wittrock (Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading. New York: Academic Press, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., & Day, R. H. Visual shape perception in early infancy. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1979, 44, (No. 182).

    Google Scholar 

  • Selz, M. Halstead—Reitan neuropsychological test batteries for children. In G. W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the school-age child: Issues and procedures. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: The University of Illinois Press, 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B. Development and the continuum of dimensional separability. Perception & Psychophysics, 1980, 28, 164–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., & Kilroy, M. C. A continuum of dimensional separability. Perception & Psychophysics, 1979, 25, 285–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stork, D. G., & Levinson, J. Z. Receptive fields and the optimal stimulus. Science, 1982, 216, 204–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, S. Against direct perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1980, 3, 373–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, W. R. A taxonomy of visual processes. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walk, R. D. Depth perception and experience. In R. D. Walk & H. L. Pick (Eds.), Perception and experience. New York: Plenum Press, 1978.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Walk, R. D. Depth perception and a laughing heaven. In A. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development: A tribute to Eleanor J. Gibson. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, A. S., Owsley, C. J., Megaw-Nyce, J., & Gibson, E. J. Detection of elasticity as an invariant property of objects by young infants. Perception, 1980, 9, 713–718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, G. Y. Visual disabilities—Their identification and relationship with academic achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1980, 13, 13–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West, R. L., & Odom, R. D. Effects of perceptual training on the salience of information in a recall problem. Child Development, 1979, 50, 1261–1264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yonas, A., Pettersen, L., & Grandrud, C. E. Infant’s sensitivity to familiar size as information for distance. Child Development, 1982, 53, 1285–1290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zusne, L. Visual perception of form. New York: Academic Press, 1970.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Melamed, L.E., Melamed, E.C. (1985). Neuropsychology of Perception. In: Hartlage, L.C., Telzrow, C.F. (eds) The Neuropsychology of Individual Differences. Perspectives on Individual Differences. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3484-0_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3484-0_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-3486-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-3484-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics