Skip to main content

A New Ideomotor Theory of Voluntary Control

  • Chapter
Experimental Slips and Human Error

Part of the book series: Cognition and Language ((CALS))

Abstract

A century ago, psychology was defined as the study of three core topics: conation (i. e., volition or will), cognition, and emotion (Hilgard, 1980). But since the advent of behaviorism and logical positivism, the first of these topics—the question of volition, or will—has been almost entirely neglected. Cognition and, to some extent, emotion have been revived by the “cognitive revolution in psychology,” which has made it possible to approach these topics as inferential constructs that can be used with scientific rigor if they are properly operation-alized (Baars, 1986; see Ekman, 1984). But until very recently, no systematic attempt has been made to approach volition with modern theoretical and empirical tools (Baars, 1988; Kimble & Perlmutter, 1970). Still, this missing element is being increasingly recognized. George A. Miller, one of the foremost leaders in the cognitive shift in psychology, has called volition one of the few “constitutive topics” of psychology—one of those central questions that define its natural domain:

[Volition] keeps coming back to me as a critically important thing. I think ‘will’ is terribly important.... As I used to say to my students, I can foul up any of your experiments by an act of will. Now why doesn’t that make ‘will’ more important than the stuff you’re studying? (in Baars, 1986, p. 221)

This chapter can be read in conjunction with my book A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness (1988), which develops in greater detail many of the points made here (see especially Chapters 7–9).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Arbib, M. A. (1980). Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In V. B. Brooks (Ed.), Handbook of physiology (Vol. 3). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1983). Conscious contents provide the nervous system with coherent, global information. In R. Davidson, G. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 3, pp. 47–76). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1987). What is conscious in the control of action? A modern ideomotor theory of voluntary control. In D. Gorfein & R. R. Hoffman (Ed.), Learning and memory: The Ebbinghaus Centennial Symposium, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basmajian, J. V. (1979). Biofeedback: Principles and practice for the clinician. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumenthal, A. L. (1977). The process of cognition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Cohen, P. R. (1982). Emotional influences in memory and thinking: Data and theory. In M. S. Clark & T. S. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and cognition (pp. 291–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchwald, J. S. (1974). Operant conditioning of brain activity—An overview. In M. M. Chase (Ed.), Operant conditioning of brain activity. Los Angeles: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drowatsky, J. N. (1975). Motor learning: Principles and practice. Minneapolis, MN: Burgess Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A, & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1979). Specializations of the human brain. Scientific American, 241(3), 180–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, P. H. (1972). Problems of organization of motor systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 4, 308–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1982). Studies of mind and brain. Boston: Reidel.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth, B. (1984). A blackboard model of control. Artificial Intelligence, 16, 1–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. (1980). The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16(2), 107–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1940). Conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890/1983). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. (Reprint, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimble, G. A., & Perlmutter, D. (1970). The problem of volition. Psychological Review, 77(5), 361–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1980). Unitization and automaticity in perception. In J. H. Flowers (Ed.), 1980 Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 53–71). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E. J., & Imber, L. G. (1979). When practice makes imperfect: Debilitating effects of overlearning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(11), 2014–2024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1978). Neuronal vs. subjective timing for a conscious sensory experience. In P. A. Buser & P. Rougeul-Buser (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience (pp. 69–82), INSERM Symposium No. 6. Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, G. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed.) New York: Basic Books. (Russian language edition, 1969.)

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1986). A programmable blackboard. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & the PDP Group, Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations. Cambridge: MIT/Bradford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1976). Semantic bias effects on the outcomes of verbal slips. Cognition, 4, 177–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1979). Effects of cognitive set upon laboratory-induced verbal (Freudian) slips. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 421–432.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mountcastle, V. B. (1978). An organizing principle for cerebral function: The unit model and the distributed system. In G. M. Edelman & V. B. Mountcastle (Eds.), The mindful brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 86(3), 215–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, P. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention and action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. Report, Center for Human Information Processing, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pani, J. R. (1982). A functionalist approach to mental imagery. Paper presented to the Psychonomic Society, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1926). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1. Psychological issues (pp. 67–89). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1982, June). Learning from absent-minded mistakes. SSRC Newsletter, 46, 19–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1983). Absent-mindedness and cognitive control. In J. Harris & P. Morris (Eds.), Everyday memory, actions and absentmindedness (pp. 113–132) New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1984a). Lapses of attention in everyday life. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 515–549). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1984b). Little slips and big disasters. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 9(2), 11–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1992). Human error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, R., & Newell, A. (1974). Knowledge and its representation in a speech understanding system. In L. W. Gregg (Ed.), Knowledge and cognition, Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, T. L., & the PDP Group (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundation. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T. (1978). The dominant action system: An information-processing approach to consciousness. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigation into the flow of experience. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 2. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L. (1984). The private personality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10(1), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Baars, B.J. (1992). A New Ideomotor Theory of Voluntary Control. In: Baars, B.J. (eds) Experimental Slips and Human Error. Cognition and Language. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1164-3_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1164-3_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-1166-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-1164-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics