Skip to main content

Error-Minimizing Mechanisms

Boosting or Editing?

  • Chapter
Experimental Slips and Human Error

Part of the book series: Cognition and Language ((CALS))

Abstract

Speech errors can have unpleasant consequences. Errors like Example 1 may be a source of amusement, but others may be more costly. Presumably, people have developed methods for minimizing the likelihood of errors and for minimizing their consequences. There are two general ways that errors can be minimized: errors can be edited, or the correct response can be boosted. Editing is defined as a reduction in the probability of an erroneous plan or the correction of an error when a mismatch with the planned output is detected. Boosting is an increase in the probability of a correct response. Consider an analogy to signal detection theory: The signal-to-noise ratio can be improved by boosting the signal (the intended action plan) or by cutting the noise (competing-action plans that may lead to errors). Enhancing the signal-to-noise ratio either way will lead to fewer errors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1977). The planning of speech: Is there semantic editing prior to speech articulation? Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J., & Mattson, M. E. (1981). Consciousness and intention: A framework and some evidence. Cognition and Brain Theory, 4, 247–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. J., Motley, M. T., & MacKay, D. G. (1975). Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Reaction time and musical expectancy: Priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 403–410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, J. D., & Diggles, V. A. (1984). Rapid error correction during human arm movements: Evidence for central monitoring. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 348–363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1980). Phonological and lexical encoding in speech production: An analysis of naturally occurring and experimentally elicited speech errors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1985). Positive feedback in hierarchical connectionist models: Applications to language production. Cognitive Science, 9, 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1977). A model of slips of the tongue. The third LACUS forum, 3, 448–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1980). Toward a unified theory of slips of the tongue. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen and hand (pp. 273–286). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S., & Reich, P. A. (1981). Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 611–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. A., & Ballard, D. H. (1982). Connectionist models and their properties. Cognitive Science, 6, 205–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C. (1986). The eccrine system and electrodermal activity. In M. Coles, E. Donchin, & S. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes and applications (pp. 51–96). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. (1968). Psycholinguistics: Experiments in spontaneous speech. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, S., & Gutowski, W. E. (1987). Neural dynamics of decision making under risk: Affective balance and cognitive-emotional interactions. Psychological Review, 94, 300–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E., & Lansman, M. (1986). Unified model of attention and problem solving. Psychological Review, 93, 446–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (Eds.), (1988). (Special issue). Journal of Memory and Language,27 (2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Laver, J. (1980). Monitoring systems in the neurolinguistic control of speech production. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen and hand (pp. 287–305). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, W. A. (1984). Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional action. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 135–170.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech. Cognition, 14, 41–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89, 483–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1987). The organization of perception and action. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattson, M. E. (1987). Sources of competing activation in action errors. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattson, M. E., Baars, B. J., & Motley, M. T. (1985, March). Wrapping up the case against purely top-down models of speech production: Evidence from induced slips of the tongue. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 2) Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meringer, R. (1908). Aus dem Leben der Sprache. Berlin: V. Behr’s Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T. (1980). Verification of “Freudian slips” and semantic prearticulatory editing via laboratory-induced spoonerisms. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen and hand (pp. 133–147). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1975). Encoding sensitivities to phonological markedness and transitional probability: Evidence from spoonerisms. Human Communication Research, 2, 353–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1976). Laboratory induction of verbal slips: A new method for psycholinguistic research. Communication Quarterly, 24, 28–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., Baars, B.J., & Camden, C. T. (1981). Syntactic criteria in prearticulatory editing: Evidence from laboratory-induced slips of the tongue. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 503–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., Camden, C. T., & Baars, B. J. (1981). Toward verifying the assumptions of laboratory-induced slips of the tongue: The output-error and editing issues. Human Communication Research, 8, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., Camden, C. T., & Baars, B. J. (1982). Covert formulation and editing of anomalies in speech production: Evidence from experimentally elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 578–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motley, M. T., Baars, B. J., & Camden, C. T. (1983). Experimental verbal slip studies: A review and an editing model of language encoding. Communication Monographs, 50, 79–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nooteboom, S. G. (1980). Speaking and unspeaking: Detection and correction of phonological and lexical errors in spontaneous speech. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen and hand (pp. 87–95). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pachella, R. G. (1974). The interpretation of reaction time in information-processing research. In B. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition (pp. 41–82). Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J., & Mycielska, K. (1982). Absent-minded? The psychology of mental lapses and everyday errors. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 1). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shrager, J., Hogg, T., & Huberman, B. A. (1987). Observation of phase transitions in spreading activation networks. Science, 236, 1092–1094.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R. (1984). Model of social inference processes. Psychological Review, 91, 392–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stemberger, J. P., Elman, J. L., & Haden, P. (1985). Interference between phonemes during phoneme monitoring: Evidence for an interactive activation model of speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 475–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Wijk, C, & Kempen, G. (1987). A dual system for producing self-repairs in spontaneous speech: Evidence from experimentally elicited corrections. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 403–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C. D. (1984). Engineering psychology and human performance (pp. 362–363). Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yellott, J. I. (1971). Correction for fast guessing and the speed-accuracy tradeoff in choice reaction time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 8, 159–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mattson, M.E., Baars, B.J. (1992). Error-Minimizing Mechanisms. In: Baars, B.J. (eds) Experimental Slips and Human Error. Cognition and Language. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1164-3_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1164-3_11

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-1166-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-1164-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics