Imagery pp 87-92 | Cite as

Notions of Consciousness and Reality

  • Imants Baruss
  • Robert J. Moore

Abstract

The researcher approaching the study of the nature of consciousness for the first time is often bewildered by the plethora of viewpoints and lack of consensus in this area of study. Why does this situation exist? The problem appears to lie in the disparity and incompatibility of firmly held ideas about the nature of consciousness and the manner in which it is to be studied. Many of these ideas are defended by their proponents, not only as the only legitimate ideas about consciousness, but as the only existent ones. What apears to be accepted in theory, but fails to be appreciated in practice, is the recognition that these various versions of consciousness are correlated with a given person’s purported experience and their personal beliefs about the nature of reality. After briefly demonstrating the disparity of views concerning consciousness, an empirical study examining the nature of the relationship between notions of consciousness and beliefs about reality is described.

Keywords

Survey Questionnaire Conceptual System Epistemological Belief Mystical Experience Transcendent Reality 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Chang, S. C. (1978). The psychology of consciousness. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 32, 105–116.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Churchland, P. S. (1983). Consciousness: The transmutation of a concept. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64, 80–95.Google Scholar
  3. Coan, R. W. (1968). Dimensions of psychological theory. American Psychologist, 23, 715–722.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Frank, J. D. (1977). Nature and functions of belief systems—Humanism and transcendental religion. American Psychologist, 32(7), 555–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Greeley, A. (1987). The “impossible”: It’s happening. Noetic Sciences Review, 2, 7–9.Google Scholar
  6. Harvey, O. J., Hunt, D. E., & Schroder, H. M. (1961). Conceptual systems and personality organization. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  7. Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
  8. James, W. (1904). Does “consciousness” exist? The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1 (18), 477–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kimble, G. A. (1984). Psychology’s two cultures. American Psychologist, 39, (8), 833–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Klein, D. B. (1984). The concept of consciousness—A Survey. Lincoln: University of Nebraska.Google Scholar
  11. Krasner, L. & Houts, A. C. (1984). A study of the “value” systems of behavioral scientists. American Psychologist, 39(8), 840–850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Mandler, G. (1985). Cognitive psychology—An Essay in cognitive science. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Merrell-Wolff, F. (1973). The philosophy of consciousness without an object—Reflections on the nature of transcendental consciousness. New York: Julian.Google Scholar
  14. Newell, R. W. (1986). Objectivity, empiricism and truth. London: Routledge & Kegal Paul.Google Scholar
  15. Osborne, J. (1981). Approaches to consciousness in North American academic psychology. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 2(3) 271–291.Google Scholar
  16. Ring, K. (1987). Near-death experiences: Intimations of immortality? In J. S. Spong (Ed.), Consciousness and survival: An interdisciplinary inquiry into the possibility of life beyond biological death. Sausalito, CA.: Institute of Noetic Sciences.Google Scholar
  17. Roberts, T. B. (1984). The concept state-of-consciousness and an SOC model of psychology. Unpublished manuscript, Northern Illinois University, Department of Learning, Development & Special Education, DeKalb.Google Scholar
  18. Sperry, R. (1986). The new mentalist paradigm and ultimate concern. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 29(3), Part 1, 413–422.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Sperry, R. W. (1987). Structure and significance of the consciousness revolution. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 8(1), 37–65.Google Scholar
  20. Thomas, L. E. & Cooper, P. E. (1980). Incidence and psychological correlates of intense spiritual experiences. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 12(l), 75–85.Google Scholar
  21. Toulmin, S. (1982). The genealogy of “consciousness.” In P. F. Secord (Ed.), Explaining human behavior. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  22. Unger, R. K., Draper, R. D., & Pendergrass, M. L. (1986). Personal epistemology and personal experience. Journal of Social Issues, 42 (2), 67–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Walsh, R. N. (1980). The consciousness disciplines and the behavioral sciences: Questions of comparison and assessment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137 (6), 663–673.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. White, P. A. (1988). Knowing more about what we can tell: “Introspective access” and causal report accuracy 10 years later. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 13–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Imants Baruss
    • 1
  • Robert J. Moore
    • 2
  1. 1.University of ReginaCanada
  2. 2.Campion CollegeUniversity of ReginaCanada

Personalised recommendations