Advertisement

The Neuropsychology of Temperament

  • Jeffrey A. Gray
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

First, a word about “neuropsychology.” This term has commonly been used in a quite restricted sense to delineate that part of psychology which is concerned with the study, in human beings, of the effects of known (even if often poorly known) structural damage to the brain. I use “neuropsychology,” in contrast, in a much wider sense, as in my book, The Neuropsychology of Anxiety (Gray, 1982a), to mean the study, quite generally, of the role played by the brain in behavioral and psychological function, whether in human or animal subjects, and whether there is structural damage to the brain or not. Since I also take it as axiomatic (and few would, I think, disagree with the axiom) that all behavioral and psychological function depends upon the activities of the brain, it follows that “neuropsychology” has a breadth which shadows that of “psychology” itself: if there is a psychology of hunger, intelligence, love, or learning French, then there is ipso facto a neuropsychology of the same.

Keywords

Entorhinal Cortex Behavioral Inhibition System Input Output Signal Behavioral Approach System Ventral Globus Pallidus 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, D.B. Brain mechanisms for offence, defense, and submission. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1979, 2, 201–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amsel, A. Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some recent history and a theoretical extention. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 306–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlyne, D.E. Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Boddy, J., Carver, A., & Rowley, K. Effect of positive and negative verbal reinforcement on performance as a function of extraversion-introversion: Some tests of Gray’s theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 1986, 7, 81–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Deutch, J.A. The structural basis of behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1964.Google Scholar
  6. Di Chiara, G., & Imperato, A. Ethanol preferentially stimulates dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1985, 115, 131–132.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Evenden, J.L., & Robbins, T.W. Increased response switching, perseveration and perseverative switching following d-amphetamine in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 1983, 80, 67–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eysenck, H.J. (Ed.), A model for personality. New York: Springer, 1981.Google Scholar
  9. Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, M.W. Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum, 1985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fowles, D. The three arousal model: Implications of Gray’s two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity and psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 1980, 17, 87–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Graeff, F.G. The anti-aversive action of drugs. In T. Thompson, P.B. Dews, & J. Barrett (Eds.). Advances in behavioural pharmacology. Vol. 6. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1987.Google Scholar
  12. Gray, J.A. Disappointment and drugs in the rat. Advancement of Science, 1967, 23, 595–605.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gray, J.A. The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1970, 8, 249–266.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gray, J.A. Learning theory, the conceptual nervous system and personality. In V.D. Nebylitsyn & J.A.Google Scholar
  15. Gray J.A. (Eds.), The biological bases of individual behaviour. New York: Academic Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  16. Gray, J.A. Elements of a two-process theory of learning. London: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  17. Gray, J.A. Drug effects on fear and frustration: Possible limbic site of action of minor tranquillizers. In L.L. Iversen & S.D. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of psychopharmacology. Vol. 8. New York: Plenum, 1977.Google Scholar
  18. Gray, J.A. A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.). A model for personality. New York: Springer, 1981.Google Scholar
  19. Gray, J.A. The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1982a.Google Scholar
  20. Gray, J.A. Précis of ‘The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1982b, 5, 469–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gray, J.A. On mapping anxiety. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1982c, 5, 506–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gray, J.A. Where should we search for biologically based dimensions of personality? Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 1983, 4, 165–176.Google Scholar
  23. Gray, J.A. The hippocampus as an interface between cognition and emotion. In H.L. Roitblat, T.G. Bever, & H.S. Terrace (Eds.). Animal cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1984.Google Scholar
  24. Gray, J.A. Anxiety and the brain: Pigments aren’t colour names. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 1985, 38, 299–300.Google Scholar
  25. Gray, J.A. The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987.Google Scholar
  26. Gray, J.A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J.N. P., Hemsley, D.R., & Smith, A.D. The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1991, 14, in press.Google Scholar
  27. Gray, J.A., & McNaughton, N. Comparison between the behavioural effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 1983, 7, 119–188.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Groves, P.M. A theory of the functional organization of the neostriatum and the neostriatal control of voluntary movement. Brain Research Reviews, 1983, 5, 109–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gupta, U. Phenobarbitone and the relationship between extraversion and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 1984, 75, 499–506.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gupta, B.S., & Gupta, U. Dextroamphetamine and individual susceptibility to reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 1984, 75, 201–206.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gupta, S., & Shukla, A.P. Verbal operant conditioning as a function of extraversion and reinforcement. British Journal of Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
  32. Hebb, D.O. Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 1955, 62, 243–259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hoebel, B.G., Hernandez, L., McLean, S., Stanley, B.G., Aulissi, E.F., Glimcher, P., & Margolin, D. Catecholamines, enkephalin, and neurotensin in feeding and reward. In B.G. Hoebel & D. Novin (Eds.). The neural basis of feeding and reward. Brunswick, GE: Haer Institute, 1981.Google Scholar
  34. Imperato, A., Mulas, A., & Di Chiara, G. Nicotine preferentially stimulates dopamine release in the limbic system of freely moving rats. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1986, 132, 337–338.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mathews, A. Anxiety and the processing of threatening information. In V. Hamilton, G.H. Bower, & N. Frijda (Eds.). Cognitive perspectives on emotion and motivation. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1988.Google Scholar
  36. Millenson, J.R. Principles of behavioral analysis. New York: Macmillan, 1967.Google Scholar
  37. Miller, N.E. Learnable drives and rewards. In S.S. Stevens (Ed.). Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.Google Scholar
  38. Mitchell, S.N., Brazell, M.P., Joseph, M.H., Alavijeh, M.S., & Gray, J.A. Regionally specific effects of acute and chronic nicotine on rates of catecholamine and indoleamine synthesis in rat brain. European Journal of Pharmacology, 1989, 167, 311–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mowrer, O.H. On the dual nature of learning: A re-interpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.” Harvard Education Review, 1947, 17, 102–148.Google Scholar
  40. Mowrer, O.H. Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Newman, J.P. Reaction to punishment in extraverts and psychopaths: Implications for the impulsive behavior of disinhibited individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 1987, 21, 464–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Oades, R.D. The role of NA in tuning and DA in switching between signals in the CNS. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 1985, 9, 261–282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Olds, J., & Olds, M. Drives, rewards, and the brain. In F. Barron, W.C. Dement, W. Edwards, H. Lindmann, L.D. Phillips, J. Olds, & M. Olds (Eds.). New directions in psychology. Vol. 2. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1965.Google Scholar
  44. Penney, J.B., & Young, A.B. GABA as the pallidothalamic neuro-transmitter: Implications for basal ganglia function. Brain Research, 1981, 207, 195–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Phillipson, O.T., & Griffiths, A.C. The topographical order of inputs to nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience, 1985, 16, 275–296.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reiman, E.M., Raichle, M.E., Butler, F.K. Hersovitch, P., & Robins, E. A focal brain abnormality in panic disorder, a severe form of anxiety. Nature, 1984, 310, 683–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rolls, E.T. Information representation, processing and storage in the brain: Analysis at the single neuron level. In R. Ritter & S. Ritter (Eds.). Neural and molecular mechanisms of learning. New York: Springer, 1986a.Google Scholar
  48. Rolls, E.T. A theory of emotion, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. In Y. Oomura (Ed.). Emotions: Neural and chemical control. Tokyo: Japan Scientific Societies Press, and Basel: Karger, 1986b.Google Scholar
  49. Rolls, E.T., & Williams, G.V. Sensory and movement-related neuronal activity in different regions of the primatestriatum. In J.S. Schneider & T.I. Kidsky (Eds.). Basal ganglia and behaviour: Sensory aspects and motor functioning. Bern: Hans Huber, 1987.Google Scholar
  50. Shapiro, S.K., Quay, H., Hogan, A., & Schwartz, K. Response perseveration and delayed responding in undersocialized aggressive conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1988, 97, 251–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Spence, J.T., & Spence, K.W. The motivational components of manifest anxiety: Drive and drive stimuli. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.). Anxiety and behavior. New York: Academic Press, 1966.Google Scholar
  52. Spielberger, C.D. The nature and measurement of anxiety. In C.D. Spielberger & R. Diaz-Guerrero (Eds.). Cross-cultural anxiety. Washington: Hemisphere, 1976.Google Scholar
  53. Stewart, J., de Wit, H., & Eikelboom, R. Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review, 1984, 91, 251–268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Swerdlow, N.R., & Koob, G.F. Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania and depression: Toward a unified hypothesis of cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1987, 10, 215–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zuckerman, M. Leaping up the phylogenetic scale in explaining anxiety: Perils and possibilities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1982, 5, 505–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D.M., & Camac, C. What lies beyond E and N? Factor analyses of scales believed to measure basic dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1988, 54, 96–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeffrey A. Gray
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyInstitute of Psychiatry, University of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations