Advertisement

Dimensions of Personality

The Biosocial Approach to Personality
  • Hans J. Eysenck
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

It would seem difficult to doubt the truth of the proposition that man is a biosocial animal (Eysenck, 1980b). There is no longer any doubt about the strong determination of individual differences in personality by genetic factors (Eaves, Eysenck, & Martin, 1989), and much progress has been made in the study of physiological, neurological, and biochemical-hormonal factors in mediating this influence (Eysenck, 1981; Zucker-man, Ballenger, & Post, 1984; Stelmack, 1981). It has been suggested that the biological aspects of personality should be identified with the concept of temperament (Strelau, 1983) and this may prove an acceptable use, although the dictionary defines the term as equivalent to personality (“the characteristic way an individual behaves, especially towards other people”). What is not in doubt is the importance of considering individual differences as an important part of scientific psychology (Eysenck, 1984) and, indeed, it has been fundamental for any proper understanding of human behavior (Eysenck, 1983). Personality is more than superficial behavioral characteristics, easily acquired and easily abandoned; it is an indispensable part of any meaningful scientific investigation in educational, industrial, clinical, social or experimental psychology (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985).

Keywords

Skin Conductance Contingent Negative Variation Pupillary Response Lemon Juice Taboo Word 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Angst, J, & Maurer-Groeli, J. Blutgruppen und Persönlichkeit. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 1974, 218, 291–300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aston-Jones, G., & Bloom, F.E. Norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats exhibit pronounced responses to non-noxious environmental stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 1981, 8, 887–900.Google Scholar
  3. Ballenger, J.C., Post, R.M., Jimmerson, D.C., Lake, C.R., Murphy, D., Zuckerman, M., & Cronin, C. Biochemical correlates of personality traits in normals: An exploratory study. Personality and Individual Differences, 1983, 4, 615–625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barrett, P., & Eysenck, S.B.G. The assessment of personality factors across 25 countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 1984, 5, 615–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blake, M.F. Relationship between circadian rhythm of body temperature and introversion-extraversion. Nature, 1967, 275, 896–897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Calhoon, L.L. Exploration into the biochemistry of sensation-seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 1988, 9, 941–949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Claridge, G.S., Donald, J., & Birchall, P.M. Drug tolerance and personality: Some implications for Eysenck’s theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 1981, 2, 153–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Colquhoun, W.P. Temperament, inspection efficiency and time of day. Ergonomics, 1960, 3, 377–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Colquhoun, W.P. Biological rhythms and human performance. London: Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  10. Conley, J.J. The hierarchy of consistency: A review and model of longitudinal findings on adult individual differences in intelligence, personality and self-opinion. Personality and Individual Differences, 1984a, 5, 11–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Conley, J.J. Longitudinal consistency of adult personality: Self-reported psychological characteristics across 45 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1984b, 47, 1325–1333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Conley, J.J. Longitudinal stability of personality traits: A multitrait-multimethod-multioccasion analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1985, 49, 1266–1282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coursey, R.D., Buchsbaum, M.S., & Murphy, D.C. Platelet MAO activity and evoked potentials in the identification of subjects biologically at risk for psychiatric disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1979, 194, 372–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Daitzman, R., & Zuckerman, M. Disinhibitory sensation-seeking personality and gonadal hormones. Personality and Individual Differences, 1980, 1, 103–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Daitzman, R., Zuckerman, M., Sammelwitz, P., & Ganjam, V. Sensation-seeking and gonadal hormones. Journal of Biosocial Science, 1978, 10, 401–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Davis, H., & Zerlin, S. Acoustic relations of the human vertex potential. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 1966, 59, 109–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Deary, I.J., Ramsay, H., Wilson, J.A., & Riad, M. Stimulated salivation: Correlations with personality and time of day effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 1988, 9, 903–909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Duffy, E. The psychosocial significance of the concept of “arousal” or “activation”. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 265–275.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Eaves, L., Eysenck, H.J., & Martin, N. Gene, culture, and personality: An empirical approach. New York: Academic Press, 1989.Google Scholar
  20. Eysenck, H. J. The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas, 1967.Google Scholar
  21. Eysenck, H. J. The structure of human personality (3rd Ed). London: Methuen, 1970.Google Scholar
  22. Eysenck, H.J. (Ed.), The measurement of personality. Lancaster: Medical and Technical Publishers, 1976.Google Scholar
  23. Eysenck, H.J. National differences in personality as related to ABO blood groups polymorphism. Psychological Reports, 1977, 41, 1257–1258.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eysenck, H.J. The biosocial model of man and the unification of psychology. In A.J. Chapman & D.M. Jones (Eds.). Models of man. Leicester: The British Psychological Society, 1980a.Google Scholar
  25. Eysenck, H.J. Man as a biosocial animal: Comments on the sociobiology debate. Political Psychology, 1980b, 2, 43–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Eysenck, H.J. (Ed.). A model for personality. New York: Springer, 1981.Google Scholar
  27. Eysenck, H.J. Is there a paradigm in personality research? Journal of Research in Personality, 1983a, 17, 369–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Eysenck, H.J. Personality as a fundamental concept in scientific psychology. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1983b, 55, 289–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Eysenck, H.J. The place of individual differences in a scientific psychology. Annals of Theoretical Psychology, 1984, 1, 233–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Eysenck, H.J. Biological dimensions of personality. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.). Handbook of personality theory and research. New York: Guilford Press, 1990.Google Scholar
  31. Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, M.W. Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum, 1985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, S.B. C. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton, 1975, San Diego: EDITS, 1975.Google Scholar
  33. Eysenck, H.J., & Martin, I. The theoretical foundations of behavior therapy. New York: Plenum, 1987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Eysenck, H.J., & Wilson, G. The psychology of sex. London: Dent, 1979.Google Scholar
  35. Eysenck, S.B.G., & Eysenck, H.J. Physiological reactivity to sensory stimulation as a measure of personality. Psychological Reports, 1967, 20, 45–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Friedman, J., & Meares, R. Cortical evoked potentials and extraversion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1979, 41, 279–286.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Frith, C.D. Habituation of the pupil size and light responses to sound. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association. San Francisco, 1977.Google Scholar
  38. Gale, A. Electroencephalographic studies of extraversion-introversion: A case study in the psycho-physiology of individual differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 1983, 4, 371–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Golding, J.F., & Richards, M. EEG spectral analysis, visual evoked potential and photic-driving correlates of personality and memory. Personality and Individual Differences, 1985, 6, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gray, J.A. A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.). A model for personality. New York: Springer, 1981.Google Scholar
  41. Haier, R.J., Robinson, D.L., Braden, W., & Williams, D. Evoked potential augmenting-reducing and personality differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 1984, 5, 293–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hebb, D.O. Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychological Review, 1955, 62, 243–259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Holmes, D.S. Pupillary response, conditioning and personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 5, 98–103.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Home, J., & Ostberg, O. Time of day effects on extraversion and salivation. Biological Psychology, 1975, 3, 301–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Klinteberg, B., Schalling, D. Edman, G., Oreland, L., & Asberg, H.E. Personality correlates of platelet monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity in female and male subjects. Neuropsychobiology, 1987, 18, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Knorring, von, L. Visual averaged evoked responses and platelet monoamine oxidase in patients suffering from alcoholism. In H. Begleiter (Ed.). Biological effects of alcoholism. New York: Plenum, 1980.Google Scholar
  47. Knorring, von, L., Mörnstad, H., Forsgren, L., & Holmgren, S. Saliva secretion rate and saliva composition in relation to extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 1986, 7, 33–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Knorring, von, L., Oreland, I.F., & Wimblad, B. Personality traits related to monoamine oxidase activity in platelets. Psychiatric Research, 1984, 12, 11–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Krishnamoorti, S.P., & Shagaas, C. Some psychological test correlates of sedation threshold. In J. Wortis (Ed.). Recent advances in biological psychiatry. New York: Plenum, 1963.Google Scholar
  50. Lacey, J.I., & Lacey, B.C. Verification and extension of the principle of autonomic response-stereotype. American Journal of Psychology, 1958, 71, 50–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Lukas, J.N. Visual evoked potential augmenting-reducing and personality: The vertex augmenter is a sensation-seeker. Personality and Individual Differences, 1987, 8, 385–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Lynn, R. Attention, arousal and the orienting reaction. New York: Pergamon, 1966.Google Scholar
  53. Maurer-Groeli, J. Blutgruppen, Persönlichkeit und Schulabschluss: Eine Untersuchung mittels FPL Schweizarische Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 1974a, 33, 407–410.Google Scholar
  54. Maurer-Groeli, J. Blutgruppen und Krankheiten. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 1974b, 218, 301–318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H.W. Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogical Clinical Neurophysiology, 1949, 1, 455–473.Google Scholar
  56. O’Gorman, J.G., & Lloyd, J.E.M. Extraversion, impulsiveness, and EEG alpha activity. Personality and Individual Differences, 1987, 8, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. O’Gorman, J.G., & Mallise, L.R. Extraversion and the EEG: II. A test of Gale’s hypothesis. Biological Psychology, 1984, 19, 113–127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Pavlov, I.P. Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press, 1927.Google Scholar
  59. Rees, L. Constitutional factors and abnormal behaviour. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.). Handbook of abnormal psychology. London: Pitman, 1973.Google Scholar
  60. Revelle, W., Anderson, K.J., & Humphreys, M.S. Empiracal tests and theoretical extentions of arousal-based thoeries of personality. In J. Strelau, & H.J. Eysenck (Eds.). Personality dimension and arousal. New York: Plenum, 1987.Google Scholar
  61. Robinson, D.L. Properties of the diffuse thalamocortical system of human personality: A direct test of Pavlovian/Eysenckian theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 1982, 3, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rothman, H.V.L. Effects of high frequencies and intersubject variability on the auditory-evoked cortical response. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 1970, 47, 569–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Routtenberg, A. Neural mechanisms of sleep: Changing view of reticular formation function. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 481–499.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Royce, J.R., & Powell, A. Theory of personality and individual differences: Factors, systems and processes. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1983.Google Scholar
  65. Saltz, E. Manifest anxiety: Have we misread the data? Psychological Review, 1970, 77, 568–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Saxton, P.M., Siegel, J., & Lukas, J.H. Visual evoked potential augmenting/reducing slopes in cats. 2. Correlations with behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 1987, 8, 511–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Schalling, D., Asberg, M., Edman, G., & Levanaer, S. Impulsivity, non-conformity, and sensation-seeking as related to biological markers for vulnerability. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 1984, 7, 746–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schalling, D., Edman, G., Asberg, M., & Oreland, L. Platelet MAO activity associated with impulsivity and aggressivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 1988, 9, 597–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Shagass, L., & Jones, A.L. A neurophysiological test for psychiatric diagnosis. Results in 750 patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1958, 114, 1002–1009.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Shagass, L., & Kerenyi, A.B. Neurophysiologic studies of personality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1958, 126, 141–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Sokolov, E.N. Perception and the conditioned reflex. London: Pergamon, 1963.Google Scholar
  72. Soskis, D.A., & Shagass, L. Evoked potential tests of augmenting-reducing. Neurosis as pathology of brain’s limbic system. Symposium on Experimental and Clinical Neurosis. Habana, Cuba, February, 1980.Google Scholar
  73. Stelmack, R.M. The psychophysiology of extraversion and neuroticism. In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.). A model for personality. New York: Springer, 1981.Google Scholar
  74. Stelmack, R.M., Achorn, E., & Michaud, A. Extraversion and individual differences in auditory evoked response. Psychophysiology, 1977, 14, 368–374.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Stelmack, R.M., Bourgeois, R.P., Chain, J., & Pickard, L.W. Extraversion and the orienting reaction habituation rate to visual stimuli. Journal of Research in Personality, 1979, 13, 49–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Stelmack, R.M., & Madelzys, N. Extraversion and pupillary response to affective and taboo words. Psychophysiology, 1975, 12, 536–546.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Stenberg, G., Rosen, I, & Risberg, J. Personality and augmenting/reducing in visual and auditory evoked potentials. Personality and Individual Differences, 1988, 9, 571–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Strelau, J. Temperament—personality—activity. London: Academic Press, 1983.Google Scholar
  79. Strelau, J., & Eysenck, H.J. (Eds.). Personality dimensions and arousal. New York: Plenum, 1987.Google Scholar
  80. Vando, A. The development of the R-A Scale: A paper-and-pencil measure of pain tolerance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1974, 1, 28–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Venturini, R., Pascalis, V. de, Imperiali, M.G., & Martini, P.S. EEG alpha reactivity and extraversion-introversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 1981, 2, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Werre, P.P. Contingent negative variation and inter-individual differences. In R. Sinz, & M.K. Rosenweig (Eds.). Psychophysiology, memory, motivation and event-related potentials in mental operations. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1983.Google Scholar
  83. Wilson, G.D. Personality, time of day and arousal. Journal of Research in Personality, in press.Google Scholar
  84. Yerkes, R.M., & Dodson, J.D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology, 1908, 18, 459–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Zuckerman, M. (Ed.). Biological bases of sensation seeking, impulsivity and anxiety. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1983.Google Scholar
  86. Zuckerman, M., Ballenger, J.C., & Post, R.M. The neurobiology of some dimensions of personality. International Review of Neurobiology, 1984, 25, 391–436.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Zuckerman, M., & Como, P. Sensation-seeking and arousal systems. Personality and Individual Differences, 1983, 4, 381–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Zuckerman, M., Murtaugh, T., & Siegel, J. Sensation-seeking and cortical augmenting/reducing. Psy-chophysiology, 1974, 11, 535–542.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Hans J. Eysenck
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Institute of PsychiatryUniversity of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations