Advertisement

Temperament

A Developmental Framework
  • Mary Klevjord Rothbart
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

In our theoretical work on temperament, Douglas Derryberry and I have defined temperament as individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation that are assumed to have a constitutional basis (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). By reactivity, we mean the arousability of multiple physiological and behavioral systems of the organism (e. g., somatic, endocrine, autonomic, and central nervous systems) as reflected in response parameters of threshold, latency, intensity, rise time and recovery time. By self-regulation, we refer to processes that act to modulate reactivity, including at the behavioral level selective attention and responsiveness to cues signaling reward and punishment, behavioral inhibition to novel or intense stimuli, and effortful control. When we say that temperament is constitutionally based, we are referring to the relatively enduring aspects of the makeup of the organism, as influenced over time by heredity, maturation and experience.

Keywords

Behavioral Inhibition Effortful Control Behavioral Inhibition System Infant Temperament Intense Stimulus 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bates, J.E. Temperament in infancy. In J.D. Osofsky (Ed.). Handbook in infant development (pp. 1101–1149). New York: Wiley, 1987.Google Scholar
  2. Bates, J.E. Concepts and measures of temperament. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 3–26). Chichester: Wiley, 1989.Google Scholar
  3. Block, J.H., & Block, J. The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W.A. Collins (Ed.). Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–101). New York: Erlbaum, 1980.Google Scholar
  4. Brackbill, Y. Cumulative effects of continuous stimulation on arousal level in infants. Child Development, 1971, 42, 17–26.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brackbill, Y. Continuous stimulation reduces arousal level: Stability of the effect over time. Child Development, 1973, 44, 43–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buss, A.H., & Plomin, R. A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley, 1975.Google Scholar
  7. Buss, A.H., & Plomin, R. Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1984.Google Scholar
  8. Capaldi, D., & Rothbart, M.K. Development of an adolescent temperament measure. Paper presented at the meetings of the Society for Research on Adolescence. Atlanta, Georgia, 1990, March.Google Scholar
  9. Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M.K. Arousal, affect and attention as components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1988, 55, 953–966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Diamond, A. Retrieval of an object from an open box. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development. Boston, MA, 1981.Google Scholar
  11. Eaton, W.O., & Yu, A.P. Are sex differences in child motor activity level a function of sex differences in maturational status? Child Development, 1989, 60, 1005–1011.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eysenck, H. J. The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, 1967.Google Scholar
  13. Eysenck, H.J. (Ed.). The measurement of personality. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  14. Eysenck, H.J., & Eysenck, M.W. Personality and individual differences. New York: Plenum, 1985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Goldsmith, H.H., & Campos, R. Toward a theory of infant temperament. In R. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.). Attachment and affiliative systems (pp. 161–193). New York: Plenum, 1982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gray, J.A. The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1982.Google Scholar
  17. Hageküll, B., & Bohlin, G. Individual stability in dimensions of infant behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 1981, 4, 97–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hageküll, B. The Baby and Toddler Questionnaires: Empirical studies and conceptual considerations. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1985, 26, 110–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kistiakovskaia, M.I. Stimuli evoking positive emotions in infants in the first months of life. Soviet Psychology and Psychiatry, 1965, 3, 39–48.Google Scholar
  20. Lawson, K.R., & Turkewitz, G. Intersensory function in newborns: Effect of sound in visual preferences. Child Development, 1980, 51, 1295–1298.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lewkowicz, D.J., & Turkewitz, G. Intersensory interaction in newborns: Modification of visual preferences following exposure to sound. Child Development, 1981, 52, 827–832.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Luria, A.R. The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York: Liveright, 1961.Google Scholar
  23. MacDonald, K. Social and personality development: An evolutionary synthesis. New York: Plenum Press, 1988.Google Scholar
  24. Matheny, A.P. Jr., Riese, M.L., & Wilson, R.S. Rudiments of infant temperament: Newborn to nine months. Developmental Psychology, 1985, 21, 486–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McCrae, R.R., & Costa, P.T., Jr. Updating Norman’s “Adequate Taxonomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1985, 49, 710–721.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McGuire, I., & Turkewitz, G. Approach-withdrawal theory and the study of infant development. In M. Bortner (Ed.). Cognitive growth and development (57–84). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1979.Google Scholar
  27. Miller, A. For your own good: Hidden cruelty in child-rearing and the roots of violence. New York: Farrar, Straus, Giroux, 1984.Google Scholar
  28. Posner, M.I., & Peterson, S.E. The attention system of the human brain. In W.M. Cowan (Ed.), Annual review of neuroscience, in press.Google Scholar
  29. Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. Intentional chapters on unintended thoughts. In J.S. Uleman & J.A. Bargh (Eds.). Unintended thought: Limits of awareness, intention and control (pp. 450–469). New York: Guilford, 1990.Google Scholar
  30. Posner, M.I. & Rothbart, M.K. Attention mechanisms and conscious experience. In M. Rugg & A.D. Milner (Eds.), Consciousness and Cognition, in press.Google Scholar
  31. Riese, M.L. Temperamental stability between the neonatal period and 24 months. Developmental Psychology, 1987, 23, 216–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Robson, K.S., & Moss, H.A. Patterns and determinants of maternal attachment. Journal of Pediatrics, 1970, 77, 976–985.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rothbart, M.K. Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 1981, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rothbart, M.K. Longitudinal observation of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 1986, 22, 356–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rothbart, M.K. Laboratory observations of the development of infant temperament. Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, 1987, April.Google Scholar
  36. Rothbart, M.K. Temperament and the development of inhibited approach. Child Development, 1988a, 59, 1241–1250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rothbart, M.K. Attention and emotion in the development of temperament. In M.I. Posner (Ed.), The role of attention in normal development and psychopathology: Proceedings of a symposium (pp. 2-6). University of Oregon Center for the Study of Emotion, Technical Report No. 88-3, 1988b.Google Scholar
  38. Rothbart, M.K. Biological Processes in temperament. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 77–110). Chichester: Wiley, 1989a.Google Scholar
  39. Rothbart, M.K. Temperament and development. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 187–248). Chichester: Wiley, 1989b.Google Scholar
  40. Rothbart, M.K. Temperament in childhood: A framework. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 59–76). Chichester: Wiley, 1989c.Google Scholar
  41. Rothbart, M.K. Behavioral approach and inhibition. In S. Reznick (Ed.) Perspectives on Behavioral Inhibition (pp. 139–157). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1989d.Google Scholar
  42. Rothbart, M.K., & Derryberry, D. Development of individual differences in temperament. In M.E. Lamb, & A.L. Brown (Eds.). Advances in developmental psychology. (Vol. 1, pp. 37–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
  43. Rothbart, M.K., & Mauro, J.A. Questionnaire measures of infant temperament. In J. Colombo & J.W. Fagen (Eds.). Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability and prediction (pp. 411–430). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1990a.Google Scholar
  44. Rothbart, M.K., & Mauro, J.A. Temperament, behavioral inhibition and shyness in childhood. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.). Handbook of social anxiety (pp. 139–160). New York: Plenum, 1990b.Google Scholar
  45. Rothbart, M.K., & Posner, M.I. Temperament and the development of self-regulation. In L.C. Hartlage, & C.F. Telzrow (Eds.). The neuropsychology of individual differences: A developmental perspective (pp. 93–123). New York: Plenum, 1985.Google Scholar
  46. Rothbart, M.K., & Ziaie, H. Infant behaviors that may modulate distress. In J.L. Gewirtz, Chair, “Development and control of infant separation distress.” Meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies, Washington, DC, 1988.Google Scholar
  47. Sanson, A., Prior, M., Garino, E., Oberklaid, F., & Sewell, J. The structure of infant temperament: Factor analysis of the Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 1987, 10, 97–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Simonov, P.V. The emotional brain. New York: Plenum, 1986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Strelau, J. J. Temperament-personality-activity. London: Academic Press, 1983.Google Scholar
  50. Strelau, J. The regulative theory of temperament as a result of east-west influences. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 35–48). Chichester: Wiley, 1989.Google Scholar
  51. Tellegen, A. Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A.H. Tuma & J.D. Maser (Eds.). Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1985.Google Scholar
  52. Thoman, E.B., Becker, P.T., & Freese, M.P. Individual patterns of mother-infant interaction. In G.P. Sackett (Ed.). Observing behavior, Volume I: Theory and applications in mental retardation (pp. 95–114). Baltimore: University Park Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  53. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1977.Google Scholar
  54. Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H.G., Hertzig, M., & Korn, S. Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York: New York University Press, 1963.Google Scholar
  55. Valsiner, J. Culture and the development of children’s action. Chichester. Wiley, 1987.Google Scholar
  56. van den Boom, D. Neonatal irritability and the development of attachment. In G. Kohnstamm, J. Bates, & M.K. Rothbart (Eds.). Temperament in childhood (pp. 299–320). Chichester: Wiley, 1989.Google Scholar
  57. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 1985, 98, 219–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Webb, E. Character and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology Monographs, I, 3, 1915.Google Scholar
  59. Worobey, J., & Blajda, V.M. Temperamental ratings at 2 weeks, 2 months, and 1 year: Differential stability of activity and emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 1989, 25, 257–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mary Klevjord Rothbart
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of OregonEugeneUSA

Personalised recommendations