Skip to main content

Dyadic Interchanges in Families with At-Risk Children

An Individual Difference Approach

  • Chapter
Early Identification of Children at Risk

Part of the book series: Topics in Developmental Psychobiology ((TDP))

  • 102 Accesses

Abstract

The study of between-group differences has a long history. In the search for explanations of behavioral differences, the emphasis has been on aggregates rather than on individuals. The focus has been on what is special about one group as compared to another, rather than on change and continuity over time within groups. Typically, the groups differ in that one represents a normative and the other a nonnormative condition. The nonnormative condition is usually one that may be characterized as “different” and/or “deficient” vis-à-vis the comparison group.

The research reported in this chapter was supported by a grant from the Bureau of Education of the Handicapped (now Office of Special Education, U.S. Department of Education).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bakeman, R., & Brown, J. (1980). Early intervention: Consequence for social and mental development at three years. Child Development, 51, 437–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1972). Temperament profile of children with Down’s Syndrome. Development and Child Neurology, 14, 640–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, L., & Cohen, S. (1980). Interactions of preterm infants with their caregivers and test performance at age 2. In T. Field, S. Goldberg, D. Stern, & A. Sostek (Eds.), High-risk infant and children: Adult and peer interactions. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bee, H. L., Barnard, K. E., Eyres, S. J., Gray, C. A., Hammond, M. A., Spietz, A. L., Snyder, C., & Clark, B. (1982). Prediction of IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child performance, family characteristics, and mother-infant interaction. Child Development, 53(5), 1134–1156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, P. B. (1980). Characteristics of handicapped infants: A study of the relationship between child characteristics and stress as reported by mothers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, S., & Ainsworth, M. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development, 43, 1171–1190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bridges, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (1982). Mothers’ ratings of the temperament characteristics of Downs syndrome infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 238–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1982a). Affective exchanges between normal and handicapped infants and their mothers. In T. Field & A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and interaction: Normal and high-risk infants. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1982b). Temperament and affective interaction in handicapped infants. Journal of the Division of Early Childhood, 5, 31–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1983). Screening and diagnosing handicapped infants. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 3(1), 14–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Lewis, M. (1984). Maternal responsivity in interactions with handicapped infants. Child Development, 55(3), 782–793.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckhalt, J. A., Rutherford, R. B., & Goldberg, K. E. (1978). Verbal and nonverbal interaction of mothers with their Down’s syndrome and nonretarded infants. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 82(4), 337–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, W. B. (1970). A simplified method for measuring infant temperament. Journal of Pediatrics, 77, 188–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, W. B. (1972). Measuring infant temperament. Journal of Pediatrics, 81 (4), 823–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chess, S., & Korn, S. (1970a). The influence of temperament of education of mentally retarded children. Journal of Special Education, 4(1), 13–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chess, S., & Korn, S. (1970b). Temperament and behavior disorders in mentally retarded children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23, 122–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1976). The relationship between affective and cognitive development in Down’s syndrome infants. Child Development, 47, 920–929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). An organization view of affect: Illustration from the study of Down’s syndrome infants. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunst, C. (1980, April). Developmental characteristics of communicative acts among Down’s syndrome infants and nonretarded infants. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Southeastern Conference on Human Development, Alexandria, Virginia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R. N., & Brown, C. (1978). Adaptation to the birth of a Down’s syndrome infant: Grieving and maternal attachment. Journal of American Academy Child Psychiatry, 17, 299–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R., Katz, E. L., & Thorpe, J. K. (1978). Emotional expression in infancy: Early deviations in Down’s syndrome. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect: Genesis of behavior (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. (1980). Supplemental stimulation of preterm neonates. Early Human Development, 4, 301–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A., & Lamb, M. (1980). Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Child Development, 51(1), 238–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, R. (1979). Positive affect in multiply handicapped infants: Its relationship to developmental age, temperament, physical status, and setting. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Kearsley, R., & Zelazo, P. (1978). Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karnes, M., Schwedel, A., Lewis, G., Ratts, D., & Esray, D. (1981). Impact of early programming for the handicapped: A follow-up study into the elementary school. Journal of the Division for Early Childhood, 4, 62–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kearsley, R. B. (1979). Iatrogenic retardation: A syndrome of learned incompetence. In R. Kearsley & I. Sigel (Eds.), Infants at risk: Assessment of cognitive functioning. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leifer, J., & Lewis, M. (1983). Maternal speech to normal and handicapped children: A look at question-asking behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 6(2), 175–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. (1972). State as an infant-environment interaction: An analysis of mother-infant interaction as a function of sex. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 95–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1982). Developmental models and assessment issues. In N. Anastasiow, W. Frankenburg, & A. Fandal (Eds.), Identifying the developmentally delayed child. Maryland: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Children’s emotions and moods: Development, theory, and measurement. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ludlow, J., & Allen, L. (1979). The effect of early intervention and preschool stimulus on the development of the Down’s syndrome child. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 23, 29–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moerk, E. (1974). Changes in verbal mother-child interaction with increasing language skills of the child. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 3, 101–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M., Fredericks, H., & Baldwin, V. (1981). The long-range effects of early childhood education on a trainable mentally retarded population. Journal of the Division for Early Childhood, 4, 92–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss, H. (1967). Sex, age, and state as determinants of mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piper, M., & Pless, I. (1980). Early intervention for infants with Down’s syndrome: A controlled trial. Pediatrics, 65, 463–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramey, C. T., & Haskins, R. (1981). The causes and treatment of school failure: Insights from the Carolina Abecedarian Project. In M. Begab, H. Garber, & H. C. Haywood (Eds.), Causes and prevention of retarded development in psychosocially disadvantaged children. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rondal, J. (1978). Maternal speech to Down’s syndrome and normal children matched for mean-length of utterance. In E. Myers (Ed.), Quality of life in severely and profoundly retarded people: Research foundations for improvement. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of care-taking casualty. In F. D. Horowitz, M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-Salapatek, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4). Chicago, IL: Univ. of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M., & Gleitman, R. (1973). The development of communication skills: Modifications in speech of young children as a function of the listener. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(5, Serial No. 152).

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. (1972). Mother’s speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Frank, M. (1981). Maternal referencing in normal and Down’s syndrome infants: A longitudinal analysis. In R. N. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.), The development of attachment and affiliative systems (pp. 281–292). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1974). Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver: The origins of behavior (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thoman, E. B. (1980). Disruption and asynchrony in early parent-infant interaction. In D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, & J. H. Penticuff (Eds.), Exceptional infant: Psychosocial risks in infant-environment transactions (Vol. 4). New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana, IL: Univ. of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vietze, P., Abernathy, S., Ashe, M., & Faulstich, G. (1977). Contingent interaction between mothers and their developmentally delayed infants. In G. Sackett (Ed.), Observing behavior: Theory and application in mental retardation (Vol. 1). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D., & Gruen, G. E. (1982). Early experience in human development. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Winick, M., & Morgan, B. (1980). Effects of administration of N-Aceylnew-raminic acid (NANA) on brain NANA content and behavior. Journal of Nutrition, 110(3), 416–424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winick, M., Meyer, K. K., & Harris, R. C. (1975). Malnutrition and environmental enrichment by early adoption. Science, 190, 1173–1175.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H. (1969). The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4). London: Metheun.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarrow, L. J., Rubenstein, J. L., & Pedersen, F. A. (1975). Infant and environment: Early cognitive and motivational development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. R. (1979). Reactivity to perceptual-cognitive events: Application for infant assessment. In R. Kearsley & I. Sigel (Eds.), Infants at risk: Assessment of cognitive functioning. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeskind, P. S., & Lester, B. M. (1978). Acoustic features and auditory perceptions of the cries of newborns with prenatal and perinatal complications. Child Development, 49, 580–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brooks-Gunn, J. (1985). Dyadic Interchanges in Families with At-Risk Children. In: Frankenburg, W.K., Emde, R.N., Sullivan, J.W. (eds) Early Identification of Children at Risk. Topics in Developmental Psychobiology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0536-9_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0536-9_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-0538-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-0536-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics