Advertisement

Early Intervention

What Does It Mean?
  • T. Berry Brazelton

Abstract

Coincident with our increasing capacity for conquering physical disease we have become sensitized to our capacity for (1) prevention of many physical and psychological disorders and (2) the mitigation of their effects on the quality of life for affected individuals. Our attitudes toward handicap and disease have been changing rapidly in the past two decades. Prevention, intervention, quality of life are becoming catchwords for the disciplines which are concerned with medical and psychological disorders. Plasticity, the capacity of a developing organism to find pathways around a deficit, has dawned upon many researchers and clinicians as if it were a new concept. Often this “new” awareness has come without a deep enough understanding of its mechanisms.

Keywords

Recovery Curve Normal Infant Alert State Grief Reaction Reflex Behavior 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Als, H. Social interaction: Dynamic matrix for developing behavioral organization. In I. C. Uzgiris (Ed.), Social interaction and communication in infancy. New Directions for child development (Vol 4). San Francisco, Calif.: Josse Bass, 1979, pp. 21–41.Google Scholar
  2. Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. Assessment of behavioral organization in a preterm and a full term infant. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1981, 20, 239–263.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Als, H., Tronick, E., Adamson, L., & Brazelton, T. B. The behavior of the full term but underweight newborn infant. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1976, 18(5), 590–602.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Als, H., Lester, B. M., & Brazelton, T. B. Dynamics of the behavioral organization of the premature infant. In T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek, S. Goldberg, & H. H. Shuman (Eds.), Infants born at risk. New York: Spectrum, 1979, pp. 173–192.Google Scholar
  5. Als, H., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. B. Analysis of face-to-face interaction in infant-adult dyads. In S. J. Suomi & G. R. Stephenson (Eds.), Social interactional analysis: Methodological issues. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1979, pp. 33–76.Google Scholar
  6. Als, H., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. B. Affective reciprocity and the development of autonomy: The study of a blind infant. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1980, 19, 22–40.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bell, R. Stimulus control of parent or caretaker behavior by offspring. Developmental Psychology, 1971, 1(4), 63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bibring, G. L., Dwyer, T. F., & Valenstein, A. F. A study of the psychological processes in pregnancy and of the earliest mother-child relationship. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1961, 16, 9–72.Google Scholar
  9. Bower, T. G. R. Visual world of infants. Scientific American, 1966, 215, 80–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss, Vol I: Attachment. New York: Basic Books, 1969.Google Scholar
  11. Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss, Vol II: Separation. New York: Basic Books, 1973.Google Scholar
  12. Brazelton, T. B. Psychophysiological reactions in the neonate. No. 1: The value of observation of the newborn. Journal of Pediatrics, 1961, 58, 508. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brazelton, T. B. Psychophysiological reactions in the neonate. No. 2: Effect of maternal medication. Journal of Pediatrics, 1961, 58, 513. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brazelton, T. B. The early mother-infant adjustment. Pediatrics, 1963, 32, 931.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brazelton, T. B. Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. Spastics International Medical Publications, Monograph #50. London: William Heinemann; Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott, 1973.Google Scholar
  16. Brazelton, T. B. Introduction. In A. Sameroff (Ed.), Organization and stability of newborn behavior: A commentary on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1978, 43, 14-29.Google Scholar
  17. Brazelton, T. B., & Als, H. Four early stages in the development of mother-infant interaction. Presented as the Helen Ross Lecture, Chicago Psychoanalytic Society, Chicago, April 1978.Google Scholar
  18. Brazelton, T. B., & Als, H. Four early stages in the development of mother-infant interaction. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1979, 34, 349–369.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Brazelton, T. B., Tronick, E., Adamson, L., Als, H., & Wise, S. Early mother-infant reciprocity. In M. A. Hofer (Ed.), Parent-infant interaction. London: Ciba, 1975, pp. 137–154.Google Scholar
  20. Carpenter, G. C. Visual regard of moving and stationary faces in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1974, 20, 181–195.Google Scholar
  21. Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech. Science, 1974, 183, 99–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cravioto, J., Delcardie, E., & Birch, H. G. Nutrition, growth and neurointegrative development. Pediatrics Supplement, 1966, 38, 319.Google Scholar
  23. Drillien, C. M. The growth and development of the prematurely born infant. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1964.Google Scholar
  24. Dubowitz, L. M., & Dubowitz, V. Clinical assessment of gestational age in the newborn infant. Journal of Pediatrics, 1970, 77, 1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Emde, R., Swedberg, J., & Suzuki, B. Human wakefulness and biological rhythms during the first postnatal hours. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1975, 35, 780–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Emde, R., Gaensbauer, T. J., & Harmon, R. J. Emotional expression in infancy. Psychological Issues X: Monograph, 37, 1976.Google Scholar
  27. Eyler, F. Demonstration of premature infants’ capabilities to improve maternal attitude and facilitate mother-infant interaction. Doctoral dissertation, University of Florida, 1979.Google Scholar
  28. Fraiberg, S. Insights from the blind. New York: Basic Books, 1977.Google Scholar
  29. Greenacre, P. On focal symbiosis. In L. Jessner & E. Pavenstedt (Eds.), Dynamic psychopathology in childhood. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1959, pp. 243–256.Google Scholar
  30. Greenberg, D. Parental reactions to an infant with a birth defect. Doctoral dissertation, Smith College, School of Social Work, 1979.Google Scholar
  31. Greenberg, N. H. A comparison of infant-mother interactional behavior in infants with atypical behavior and normal infants. In J. Hellmuth (Ed.), Exceptional infant: Studies in abnormalities (Vol. II). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1971, p. 390.Google Scholar
  32. Harlow, H. F. Love in infant monkeys. Scientific American, 1959, 200, 68–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. R. Affectional responses in infant monkeys. Science, 1959, 130, 421–432.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Heider, G. M. Vulnerability in infants and young children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1966, 73, 1.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kagan, J., Kearsley, R., & Zelazo, P. Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  36. Klaus, M. H., & Kennell, J. H. Mothers separated from their newborn infants. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 1970, 17, 1015.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Klein, R. E., Habicht, J. P., & Yarbrough, C. Effect of protein calorie malnutrition on mental development. Incap Publication, 1971, No. 1, p. 571.Google Scholar
  38. Lester, B. M. Assessment of the behavioral organization of the neonate: Research and clinical perspectives. Paper presented as part of the Joint Colloquium Series between the School of Education and the Program of Developmental Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, November 6, 1979.Google Scholar
  39. Lester, B. M. Issues in neonatal behavioral assessment. In E. Sell (Ed.), Follow-up of the high risk newborn. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1980, pp. 291–295.Google Scholar
  40. Lindemann, E. Grief. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1944, 101, 141.Google Scholar
  41. Lubchenco, L. O., Hansman, C, Dressier, M., & Boyd, E. Intrauterine growth estimated from liveborn, birthweight data at 24 to 42 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics, 1963, 32, 793.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 1977, 198, 75–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Miller, H. C, & Hassanien, K. Diagnosis of impaired fetal growth in newborn infants. Pediatrics, 1971, 43, 511–515.Google Scholar
  44. Neligan, G. A., Kolvin, I., Scott, D., & Garside, R. F. Born too soon or born too small. Spastics International Medical Publications. London: William Heinemann, 1976.Google Scholar
  45. Parmelee, A. J., & Michaelis, R. Neurological examination of the newborn. In J. Hellmuth (Ed.), Exceptional infant: Studies in abnormalities (Vol. II). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1971.Google Scholar
  46. Provence, S., & Lipton, R. C. Infants in institutions. New York: International Universities Press, 1962.Google Scholar
  47. Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In F. D. Horowitz, M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-Salapatek, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4). Chicago: University Press, 1975, pp. 187–244.Google Scholar
  48. Sander, L. W. The regulation of exchange in the infant-caregiver system and some aspects of the context-content relationship. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley, 1977, pp. 137–156.Google Scholar
  49. Sigman, M., & Parmelee, A. H. Longitudinal evaluation of the preterm infant. In T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek, S. Goldberg, & H. H. Shuman (Eds.), Infants born at risk. New York: Spectrum, 1979, pp. 193–219.Google Scholar
  50. Spitz, R. Hospitalization: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1945, 1, 53–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Stechler, G., & Latz, M. A. Some observations on attention and arousal in the human neonate. American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1966, 5, 517–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. B. Clinical uses of the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment. In B. Z. Friedlander, G. M. Sterritt, & G. E. Kirk (Eds.), Exceptional infant: Assessment and intervention (Vol III). New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1975, p. 137.Google Scholar
  53. Tronick, E., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. Mutuality in mother-infant interaction. Journal of Communication, 1977, 7, 74–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., & Brazelton, T. B. The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1978, 17, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Warkany, J., Monroe, B., & Sutherland, B. Intrauterine growth retardation. American Journal of Diseases of the Child, 1961, 102, 127.Google Scholar
  56. White, R. W. Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 1959, 66, 297–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • T. Berry Brazelton
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Child Development UnitChildren’s Hospital Medical CenterBostonUSA
  2. 2.Harvard Medical SchoolBostonUSA

Personalised recommendations