Temperament—Intelligence Reciprocities in Early Childhood

A Contextual Model
  • Richard M. Lerner
  • Jacqueline V. Lerner

Abstract

In the more than quarter century since Alexander Thomas and Stella Chess initiated a longitudinal study of the implications of individual differences in temperament for psychosocial adaptation—the New York Longitudinal Study—there has been considerable interest in the study of temperament among psychiatrists, developmental psychologists, and pediatricians. In part, this interest derives from the fact that the Thomas and Chess approach to the study of temperament was both a product and a producer of the contextual zeitgeist that has characterized American social science for more than a decade (e.g., Jenkins, 1974; Lerner, Hultsch, & Dixon, in press; Mischel, 1977; Sarbin, 1977).

Keywords

Temperament Attribute Psychosocial Adaptation Infant Temperament Difficult Child Behavioral Style 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bandura, A. The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 344–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. Self-referent thought: A developmental analysis of self-efficacy. In J. H. Flavell & L. D. Ross (Eds.), Cognitive social development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1980.Google Scholar
  3. Bandura, A. The self and mechanisms of agency. In J. Suls (Ed.), Social psychological perspectives on the self. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1980.Google Scholar
  4. Baron, J. Temperament profile of children with Down’s syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1972, 14, 640–643.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bates, J. E. The concept of difficult temperament. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1980, 26, 299–319.Google Scholar
  6. Bell, R. Q. A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 81–95.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bell, R. Q. Contributions of human infants to caregiving and social interaction. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  8. Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. Physical attractiveness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  9. Buss, A., & Plomin, R. A temperamental theory of personality development. New York: Wiley, 1975.Google Scholar
  10. Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 1978, 61, 735–739.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Carey, W. B., Fox, M., & McDevitt, S. C. Temperament as a factor in early school adjustment. Pediatrics, 1977, 60, 621–624.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Chess, S., & Korn, S. Temperament and behavior disorders in mentally retarded children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1970, 23, 122–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Darwin, C. The origin of species. London: John Murray, 1959.Google Scholar
  14. Dunn, J. F. Individual differences in temperament. In M. Rutter (Ed.), The scientific foundations of developmental psychiatry. London: Heinemann Medical Books, 1980.Google Scholar
  15. Field, T., Hallock, N., Ting, G., Dempsey, J., Dabiri, C., & Shuman, H. H. A first-year follow-up of high-risk infants: Formulating a cumulative risk index. Child Development, 1978, 49, 119–131.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. Toward a theory of infant temperament. In R. N. Emde & R. Harmon (Eds.), Attachment and affiliative systems: Neurobiological and psychobiological aspects. New York: Plenum Press, 1981.Google Scholar
  17. Gordon, E. M., & Thomas, A. Children’s behavioral style and the teacher’s appraisal of their intelligence. Journal of School Psychology, 1967, 5, 292–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hall, G. S. Adolescence. New York: Appleton, 1904.Google Scholar
  19. Hull, C. L. A behavior system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1952.Google Scholar
  20. Jenkins, J. J. Remember that old theory of memory? Well forget it. American Psychologist, 1974, 29, 785–795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kagan, J. Reflection-impulsivity: The generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1966, 71, 17–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kendall, P. Cognitive-behavioral interventions with children. In B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in child clinical psychology (Vol. 4 ). New York: Plenum Press, 1981.Google Scholar
  23. Korn, S. Temperament, vulnerability, and behavior. Paper presented at the Louisville Temperament Conference, Louisville, Kentucky, September 1978.Google Scholar
  24. Lerner, J. V. The role of temperament in psychosocial adaptation in early adolescents: A test of a “goodness of fit” model. Journal of Genetic Psychology,in press.Google Scholar
  25. Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. Temperament and adaptation across life: Theoretical and empirical issues. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press, in press.Google Scholar
  26. Lerner, J. V., Lerner, R. M., & Zabski, S. Temperament and elementary school children’s actual and rated academic performance: A test of a “goodness of fit” model. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,in press.Google Scholar
  27. Lerner, R. M. Nature, nurture, and dynamic interactionism. Human Development, 1978, 21, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lerner, R. M. A dynamic interactional concept of individual and social relationship development. In R. L. Burgess & T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in developing relationships. New York: Academic Press, 1979.Google Scholar
  29. Lerner, R. M. Concepts of epigenesis: Descriptive and explanatory issues: A critique of Kitchner’s comments. Human Development, 1980, 23, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. Individuals as producers of their development: Conceptual and empirical bases. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press, 1981.Google Scholar
  31. Lerner, R. M., & Miller, R. D. Relation of students’ behavioral style to estimated and measured intelligence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1971, 33, 11–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lerner, R. M., & Spanier, G. B. (Eds.). Child influences on marital and family interaction: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  33. Lerner, R. M., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. Contextualism and the character of developmental psychology in the 1970s. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, in press.Google Scholar
  34. Lerner, R. M., Palermo, M., Spiro, A., Iii, & Nesselroade, J. R. Assessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life-span: The Dimensions of Temperament Survey (Dots). Child Development, 1982, 53, 149–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lewis, M., & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.). The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  36. Lewontin, R. C., & Levins, R. Evolution. Enciclopedia, V: Divino-Fame. Torino, Italy: Einaudi, 1978.Google Scholar
  37. Matheny, A. P., JR. Bayleÿ s infant behavior record: Behavioral components and twin nalyses. Child Development. 1980, 51, 1157–1167.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. McCall, R. B., Parke, R. D., & Kavanaugh, R. D. Imitation of live and televised models by children one to three years of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,1977, 42(5, Serial No. 173).Google Scholar
  39. McDevitt, S. C., & Carey, W. B. The measurement of temperament in 3–7 year old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1978, 19, 245–253.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mead, G. H. Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mischel, W. On the future of personality measurement. American Psychologist, 1977, 32, 246–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Palermo, M. E. Child temperament and contextual demands: A test of the goodness-of-fit model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University, 1982.Google Scholar
  43. Piaget, J. The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1950.Google Scholar
  44. Piaget, J. Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. 1 ). New York: Wiley, 1970.Google Scholar
  45. Piaget, J. Biology and knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  46. Piaget, J. Behavior and evolution. New York: Pantheon Books, 1978.Google Scholar
  47. Plomin, R., & Foch, T. T. A twin study of objectively assessed personality in childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 680–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. Development of individual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1 ). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1981.Google Scholar
  49. Sameroff, A. J. Infant risk factors in developmental deviancy. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Association for Child Psychiatry and Allied Professions, Philadelphia, July 1974.Google Scholar
  50. Sameroff, A. J. Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development, 1975, 18, 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sarbin, T. R. Contextualism: A world view for modern psychology. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1976. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  52. Schneirla, T. C. The concept of development in comparative psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  53. Snyder, M. On the influence of individuals on situations. In N. Cantor & J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Cognition, social interaction, and personality. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1981.Google Scholar
  54. Spanier, G. B., Lerner, R. M., & Aquilino, W. Future perspectives on child-family interactions. In R. M. Lerner & G. B. Spanier (Eds.), Child influences on marital and family interaction: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  55. Strelau, J. A diagnosis of temperament by nonexperimental techniques. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 4, 97–105.Google Scholar
  56. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1977.Google Scholar
  57. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. The dynamics of psychological development. New York: Brunner/Mazel, 1980.Google Scholar
  58. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. The role of temperament in the contributions of individuals to their development. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Individuals as producers of their development: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press, 1981.Google Scholar
  59. Thomas, A., Chess, S., Birch, H., Hertzig, M., & Korn, S. Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York: New York University Press, 1963.Google Scholar
  60. Thomas, A., Chess, S., Sillan, J., & Mendez, O. Cross-cultural study of behavior in children with special vulnerabilities to stress. In D. F. Ricks, A. Thomas, & M. Roff (Eds.), Life history research in psychopathology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1974.Google Scholar
  61. White, S. H. The learning-maturation controversy: Hall to Hull. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1968, 14, 187–196.Google Scholar
  62. Wohlwill, J. F. The physical and the social environment as contrasting environmental modes relevant to the development of the child. In D. Magnusson & V. Allen (Eds.), Human development: An interactional perspective. New York: Academic Press, in press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Richard M. Lerner
    • 1
  • Jacqueline V. Lerner
    • 1
  1. 1.College of Human DevelopmentThe Pennsylvania State UniversityUniversity ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations