Skip to main content

A Cognitive Neuropsychological Framework for Assessing Reading Disorders

  • Chapter
Book cover Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment

Part of the book series: Critical Issues in Neuropsychology ((CINP))

Abstract

Reading is a learned skill that engages a complex set of cognitive procedures. By virtue of its complexity, it is vulnerable to disruption with many different kinds of brain damage. Reading impairment is characteristic of almost all patients with acquired aphasia (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983), and reading disorders persist chronically in the overwhelming majority of these aphasic patients (Webb & Love, 1983). Detailed assessment of the pattern of acquired reading disturbance, or alexia, can be informative about the underlying neural dysfunction as well as the integrity of various component cognitive neuropsychological processes. Since reading is fundamentally a linguistic activity and is based on prior mastery of auditory language skills, it is possible to learn about the integrity and function of various components of the language processing system through careful analysis of reading performance. Reading also depends on visual processing. Visual letter and word identification pose many of the same problems as other types of patterns for the visual recognition system, so that the functions of certain visual processing mechanisms may also he revealed in the analysis of reading performance. Reading competence is a prerequisite for many adult functional activities; hence, comprehensive assessment of reading capabilities is important for predicting functional living skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Albert, M. L. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology, 23, 658–664.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W., and Baron, J. (1977). How children get meaning from printed words. Child Development, 48, 586–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, M. F., and Derouesne, J. (1979). Phonological alexia: Three dissociations. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 42, 1115–1124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Black, S. E., and Bub, D. (1990). The evolution of pure alexia: A longitudinal study of recovery. Brain and Language, 39, 405–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, J. R., and Spreen O. (1989). Predicting premorbid IQ: A revision of the National Adult Reading Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3. 129–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., Howard, D., and McLoughlin, P. (1984). The semantic deficit in aphasia: The relationship between semantic errors in auditory comprehension and picture naming. Neuropsvchologia, 22, 409–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Berndt, R. S., and Basili, A. G. (1983). The selective impairment of phonological processing: A case study. Brain and Language, 18, 128–174.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A review of the syndrome. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, and J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 22–47 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1981). Disorders of reading and their implications for models of normal reading. Visible Language, 15, 245–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., and Funnell, E. (1987). Reading and writing: One lexicon or two. In A. Allport, D. G. Mackay, W. Prinz, and E. Scheerer (Eds.), Language perception and production (pp. 313–339 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., and Marshall, J. C. (1987). Deep dyslexia since 1980. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, and J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 407–451 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • DePartz, M. P. (1986). Re-education of a deep dyslexic patient: Rationale of the method and results. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 149–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. W., Flude, B. M., and Young, A. W. (1987). “Neglect dyslexia” and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 439–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B. (1981). Preservation of orthographic knowledge in aphasia. Brain and Language, 14, 307–314.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B. (1988). Acquired alexia. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 1, pp. 377–391 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B. (in press). Two types of phonological alexia. Cortex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Alexander, M. P. (1984). Pictures, images and pure alexia: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 9–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., Beeman, M., Lott, S. N., Link, K., Grafman, J., and Robinson. S. (1993). Modality-specific phonological alexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 549–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., Ferguson, S., Robinson, S., and Sunderland, T. (1992). Dissociation of mechanisms of reading in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 43, 400–413.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Glosser, G. (1990). The structure of the semantic lexicon: Implications for a theory of deep alexia. Presented at the Academy of Aphasia, Baltimore, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Hadley, J. A. (1992). Letter-by-letter surface alexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 9, 185–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Kohn, S. E. (1990). Impaired activation of the phonological lexicon: Effects upon oral reading. Brain and Language, 38, 278–297.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Perlman, M. B. (1982). On the underlying causes of semantic paralexias in a patient with deep dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 20, 559–568.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., and Robinson, S. R. (1991). Whole-word training therapy in a stable surface alexic patient: It works. Aphasiology, 5, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. B., Ween, J. E., and Albert, M. L. (1993). Alexia. In K. M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology ( 3rd ed., pp. 37–62 ). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1973). The contribution of operativity to naming in aphasic patients. Neuropsychologia, 11, 213–220.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H., and Zurif, E. (1975). “Bee” but not “be”; oral reading of single words in aphasia and alexia. Neurops-r’chology, 13, I81–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glosser, G., and Friedman, R. B. (1990). The continuum of deep/phonological alexia. Cortex, 26, 343–359.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glosser, G., and Friedman. R. B. (1991). Lexical but not semantic priming in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 6, 522–527.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glosser, G., Roeltgen, D., and Friedman, R. B. (1991). Hyperlexia: A case of reading without meaning..Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., and Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hersen, M., and Barlow, D. H. (1976). Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying behavior change. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huff, F. J., Corkin, S., and Growden, J. H. (1986). Semantic impairment and anomia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 28, 235–249.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W., and Evett, L. J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, S., and Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J., and Marcel, A. (1981). One process, not two, in reading aloud: Lexical analogies do the work of non-lexical rules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 397–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay, J., and Patterson, K. E. (1985). Routes to meaning in surface dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, and M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 79–104 ). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. (1962). A variety of reading disability associated with right hemisphere lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 25, 339–344.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • LaBarge, D.. and Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaPointe, L. L., and Horner. J. (1979). Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia. Tigard, OR: C. C. Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lott, S. N., Friedman, R. B., and Linebaugh, C. W. (1994). Rationale and efficacy of a tactile-kinesthetic treatment for alexia. Aphasiology, 8, 181–195

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W. H., and MacGinitie, R. K. (1989). Gates-MacGinities Reading Tests. Chicago: Riverside Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall. J. C. (1984). Towards a rational taxonomy of the acquired dyslexics. In R. N. Malatesha and H. A. Whitaker (Eds.). Dyslexia: A global issue (pp. 211–232 ). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. C., and Newcombe, F. (1977). Variability and constraint in acquired dyslexia. In H. Whitaker and H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 3, pp. 257–286 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A.. and Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease: The breakdown of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19, 124–141.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1976). Preliminary letter identification in the perception of words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 80–91.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., and Patterson, K. E. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation, or, an attempt at a new interpretation. In M. Coltheart. K. Patterson. and J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 91–118 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nebes, R. D. (1989). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 377–394. Nelson, H. E. (1982). National Adult Reading Test. Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, H. E., and O’Connell, A. (1978). Dementia: The estimation of premorhid intelligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex, 14, 234–244.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ober, B. A., and Shenaut. G. K. (1990). Automatic versus controlled priming in Alzheimer’s disease: A synthesis of current findings. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychologv, 12, 79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E. (1982). The relation between reading and phonological coding: Further neuropsychological observation. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.). Normality and pathology in cognitive functioning (pp. 77–11 I ). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E., and Kay. J. (1982). Letter-by-letter reading: Psychological description of a neurological syndrome. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 41 I - 441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., and Coltheart, M. (Eds.). (1985). Surface dvsJeviu. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, E. M., Bogy°. L. C.. Schwartz, M. F.. and Marin. O. S. M. (1980). Does deep dyslexia reflect right hemisphere reading’? In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, and J. C. Marshall (Eds.). Deep dvs/esia (pp. 381–406 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D. C., and Ajax, E. I. (1980). Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology, 30. 509–517.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., and Marin, O. S. M. (1980). Fractionating the reading process in dementia: Evidence for word-specific print-to-sound associations. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, and J. C. Marshall (Eds.). Deep dyslexia (pp. 259–269 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., and McClelland. J. L,. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Reviewr, 96, 523–568.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., and Warrington, E. K. (1977). The possible role of selective attention in acquired dyslexia. Neuropsrchologia, 15, 31–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., and Warrington, E. K. (1980). Simple and multiple component central dyslexic syndromes. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, and J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 119–145 ). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K., and McCarthy, R. 119831. Reading without semantics. Quarter/y./ourna/ of Experimental Psychology, 35A, III - 138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D.. and Lieberman, I. W. (1972). Misreading: A search for cues. In J. F. Kavanagh and I. G. Mattingly (Eds.). Language by ear and by eve. Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiepers, C. (1980). Response latency and accuracy ni visual word recognition. Perception and Psy- chophysic.s, 27, 71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sieroff, E., Pollatsek, A.. and Posner, M. I. (1988). Recognition of visual letter strings following injury to the posterior visual attention system. Cognitive Neurop.svchology, 5, 427 449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. ’[he Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, M. A., and Farah, M. J. (1990). l’urc ale.ria as a visual impairment: A reconsideration. Presented at the Academy of Aphasia, Baltimore. Ml).

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., and Shallice. T. (1980). Word-form dyslexia. Brain. /0i. 99 112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, W. G., and Love, R. J. (1983). Reading problems in chronic aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 164–171.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, S., and Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioral neurology. In M. M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of behavioral neurology (pp. 71–123 ). Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J. L.. and Bryant, B. R. (19861. Gray Oral Reading Tests Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised. Circle Pines. MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., and Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Tests o/ Achievement. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Glosser, G., Friedman, R.B. (1995). A Cognitive Neuropsychological Framework for Assessing Reading Disorders. In: Mapou, R.L., Spector, J. (eds) Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment. Critical Issues in Neuropsychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9709-1_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9709-1_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-9711-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-9709-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics