Advertisement

Physiological Measures of Auditory Sensitivity

Near-Threshold Intensity Effects
  • W. Keith Berg
Part of the Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect book series (ASCA, volume 10)

Abstract

The scientist interested in evaluating the auditory sensitivity of young infants is presented with a difficult choice. On the one hand, physiological measures appear very attractive. There are a wide variety of them to choose from; they often can be recorded while the infant is asleep as well as awake; and the response systems are typically more mature than most externally observable motor systems. This latter quality is important because it allows the assessment of sensory-system development to be at least partially isolated from response-system development. On the other hand, physiological measures are often viewed as categorically different from behavioral measures—especially the traditional ones used with older children and adults. These effects have led Trehub and colleagues (Schneider, Trehub, & Bull, 1979; Trehub, Schneider, & Bull, 1981), and others before them, to conclude that psychophysiological techniques are sensitive to the “significance” of the stimulus and therefore track what they refer to as the “attentional threshold” rather than the threshold of audibility.

Keywords

Auditory Stimulus Stimulus Intensity Pure Tone Physiological Measure Skin Conductance 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Berg, W. K., & Berg, K. M. Psychophysiological development in infancy: State, sensory function and attention. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development. New York: Wiley, 1979.Google Scholar
  2. Berg, W. K., Jackson, J. C., & Graham, F. K. Tone intensity and rise-decay time effects on cardiac responses during sleep. Psychophysiology, 1975, 12, 254–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Eisenberg, F. K. Auditory competence in early life. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  4. Eisenberg, R. B., Griffin, E. J., Coursin, D. B., & Hunter, M. A. Auditory behavior in the human neonate: A preliminary report. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1964, 7, 245–269.Google Scholar
  5. Graham, F. K. Distinguishing among orienting, defence, and startle reflexes. In H. D. Kimmel, E. H. Van Olst, & J. F. Orlebeke (Eds.), The orienting reflex in humans. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1979.Google Scholar
  6. Graham, F. K., & Slaby, D. Differential heart rate changes to equally intense white noise and tone. Psychophysiology, 1973, 10, 347–362.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Graham, F. K., Anthony, B. J. & Zeigler, B. L. The orienting response and developmental processes. In D. Siddle (Ed.), Orienting and habituation: Perspectives in human research. Sussex, England: Wiley, in press.Google Scholar
  8. Graham, F. K., & Clifton, R. K. Heart rate change as a component of the orienting response. Psychological Bulletin, 1966, 65, 304–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hartley, H. V., & Hetrick, R. D. Ambiguities in visual identification of responses in respiration audiometry. Journal of Auditory Research, 1973, 13, 305–312.Google Scholar
  10. Hoffman, H. S., & Ison, J. R. Reflex modification in the domain of startle: I. Some empirical findings and their implications for how the nervous system processes sensory input. Psychological Review, 1980, 87, 175–189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Jackson, J. C. Amplitude and habituation of the orienting reflex as a function of stimulus intensity. Psychophysiology, 1974, 11, 647–659.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Pool, R., Goetzinger, C. P., & Rousey, C. L. A study of the effects of auditory stimuli. Acta Otolaryngologica, 1966, 61, 143–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Reiter, L. A., & Ison, J. R. Inhibition of the human eyeblink reflex: An evaluation of the sensitivity of the Wendt-Yerkes method for threshold detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 1977, 3, 325–336.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Rousey, C. L., & Reitz, W. E. Respiratory changes at auditory and visual thresholds. Psychophysiology, 1967, 3, 258–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Rousey, C., Snyder, C., & Rousey, C. Changes in respiration as a function of auditory stimuli. Journal of Auditory Research, 1964, 4, 107–114.Google Scholar
  16. Schneider, B. A., Trehub, S. E., & Bull, D. The development of basic auditory processes in infants. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1979, 33, 306–319.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Schulman, C. A. Heart rate auditometry. Part I. An evaluation of heart rate response to auditory stimuli in newborn hearing screening. Neuropadiatrie, 1973, 4, 362–374.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Sokolov, E. N. Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan, 1963.Google Scholar
  19. Trehub, S. E., Schneider, B. A., & Bull, D. Effect of reinforcement on infants’ performance in an auditory detection task. Developmental Psychology, 1981, 17, 872–877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Turkewitz, G., Birch, H. G., & Cooper, K. K. Patterns of response to different auditory stimuli in the human newborn. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 1972, 14, 487–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Turkewitz, G., Birch, H. G., & Cooper, K. K. Responsiveness to simple and complex auditory stimuli in the human newborn. Developmental Psychobiology, 1972, 5, 7–19.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • W. Keith Berg
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of FloridaGainesvilleUSA

Personalised recommendations