Skip to main content

What Is Necessarily True in Psychology?

  • Chapter
Annals of Theoretical Psychology

Abstract

The position is taken that psychology is not an empirical science and that generally valid propositions in psychology are explications of common sense and hence necessarily true. A proposition in a given context belongs to common sense if and only if all competent users of the language involved agree that the proposition in the given context is true and that its negation is contradictory or senseless. Studies attempting to test necessarily true propositions are labelled pseudoempirical. The paper presents numerous examples of necessarily true propositions and pseudoempirical studies taken from various fields of psychology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abelson, R. P., & Rosenberg, M. J. Symbolic psychologic: A model of attitudinal cognition. Behavioral Science, 1958, 3, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1978, 87, 49–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apel, K. O. Die Entfaltung der ‘sprachanalytischen’ Philosophie und das Problem der ‘Geisteswissenchaften’. Philosophische Jahrbuch, 1965, 72, 239–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 19–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. On distinguishing between logical and empirical verification. A comment on Smedslund. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1978, 19, 97–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. G., Jackson, D., Haley, J., & Weakland, J. Toward a theory of schizophrenia, Behavioral Science, 1956, 1, 251–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Speckart, G. Models of attitude-behavior relations. Psychological Review, 1979, 86, 452–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohm, D. Wholeness and the implicate order. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R., & Swartz, N. Possible worlds. An introduction to logic and its philosophy. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1978, 2, 173–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. Sprachanalysen und Verhaltenserklärungen in der Psychologie. Trierer Psychologische Berichte, 1980, 7, 3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chein, I. The science of behavior and the image of man. New York: Basic Books, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. J. Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1981, 3, 317–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., Sears, R. R., Ford, C. S., Hovland, C. I., & Sollenberger, R. T. Trustration and aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1939.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, W. Decision making. Psychological aspects. In D. L. Sills (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences (Vol. 4). New York: Macmillan, 1968, pp. 34–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St. B. T. Current issues in the psychology of reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 1980, 71, 227–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feffer, M., & Suchotliff, L. Decentering implications of social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 415–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, Ill.: Row, Peterson, 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Wohlwill, J. F. Formal and functional aspects of cognitive development. In D. Elkind & J. H. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in cognitive development. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 36, 309–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergen, K. J. Social psychology, science and history. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1976, 2, 373–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley, 1958.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Henle, M. On the relation between logic and thinking. Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 366–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israel, J. The language of dialectics and the dialectics of language. Copenhagen: Munksgaard, 1979.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ittelson, W. H. The Ames demonstrations in perception. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laucken, U. Naive Verhaltenstheorie. Stuttgart: Klett, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, E. Transfer of verbal paired associates. Psychological Review, 1965, 72, 327–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P. E. On the circularity of the law of effect. Psychological Bulletin, 1950, 47, 52–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T. Personal deserving versus justice for others: An exploration of the justice motive. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1977, 13, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Heise, G. A., & Lichten, W. The intelligibility of speech as a function of the context of the test materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1951, 41, 329–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. The similarity paradox in human learning. A resolution. Psychological Review, 1949, 56, 132–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., & Tannenbaum, P. H. The principle of congruity in the prediction of attitude change. Psychological Review, 1955, 62, 42–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ossorio, P. G. Never smile at a crocodile. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 1973, 3, 121–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ossorio, P. G., & Davis, K. E. The self, intentionality, and reactions to evaluations of the self. In C. Gordon & K. J. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social interaction. New York: Wiley, 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness and attributional style in depression. Journal of the Norwegian Psychological Association, 1981, 18, 1–18, 53–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1952.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. The psychology of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postman, L. The history and present status of the Law of Effect. Psychological Bulletin, 1947, 44, 489–563.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. J., Hovland, C. I., McGuire, W. J., Abelson, R. P., & Brehm, J. W. Attitude organization and change. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D. J. Social psychology. London: Addison-Wesley, 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuman, A. I. The double bind hypothesis a decade later. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68, 409–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. Collected papers. I. The problem of social reality. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A., & Luckmann, T. The structure of the life-world. London: Heinemann Educational Books, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shotter, J. Critical notice: Are Fincham & Shultz’s findings empirical findings? British Journal of Social Psychology, 1981, 20, 143–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shotter, J., & Newson, J. An ecological approach to cognitive development. In G. Butterworth & P. Light (Eds.), Social cognition: Studies of the development of understanding. Sussex: Harvester, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Concrete reasoning: A study of intellectual development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1964, 29, No. 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Circular relation between understanding and logic. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1970, 11, 217–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Becoming a psychologist. Theoretical foundations for a humanistic psychology. New York: Halsted Press and Oslo: Universitetsforlaget, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Bandura’s theory of self-efficacy: A set of common sense theorems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1978, 19, 1–14. (a)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Some psychological theories are not empirical: Reply to Bandura. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1978, 19, 101–102. (b)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Between the analytic and the arbitrary: A case study of psychological research. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1979, 20, 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. Analyzing the primary code: From empiricism to apriorism. In D. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. Essays in honor of J. S. Bruner. New York: Norton, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. The logic of psychological treatment. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1981, 22, 65–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. A. A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, 48, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. Human learning. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1931.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 327–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Waerden, B. L. Science awakening. Groningen, Holland: Noordhoff, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J. H., & Jacson, D. D. Pragmatics of human communication. New York: Norton, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weakland, H. H. The double bind hypothesis by self-reflexive hindsight. Family Process, 1974, 13, 269–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1984 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Smedslund, J. (1984). What Is Necessarily True in Psychology?. In: Royce, J.R., Mos, L.P. (eds) Annals of Theoretical Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9191-4_21

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9191-4_21

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-9193-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-9191-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics