Skip to main content

Individual Differences in Environmental Exploration and Cognitive Mapping Skills

Early Development in Social Contexts

  • Chapter
Action in Social Context

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Developmental Psychology ((PDPS))

Abstract

Parents can strongly influence their children’s environmental experiences, as these examples illustrate. From the time infants become mobile, parents provide them with environments to explore, set the boundaries for exploration, structure the spatial layouts and objects provided in the home environment, encourage and discourage certain types of exploration and point out spatial locations and relationships. To the extent that they influence their children’s exploration and use of environments, parents probably also influence the development of their children’s cognitive mapping skills, that is, their skills for maintaining spatial orientation and solving spatial problems in large-scale environments.

[The] effective child-rearer makes the living area as safe as possible for the naive newly crawling or walking child and then provides maximum access to the living area for the child. This immediately sets the process of development off in a manner that will lead naturally to the satisfaction of and further development of the child’s curiousity; the opportunity to learn about the world at large. (White, Kaban, Shapiro, & Attonucci, 1976, pp. 150–151)

Davy’s father shares the route he chooses to take and consciously develops skills of observation in his son while they journey to his work places together. Both of his parents talk with their children continually wherever they drive; they impart curiousity about places....This is part of his father’s educational philosophy, one which can best be termed as an apprenticeship in practical education: Davy was being trained to be competent at hunting, finding objects and using them resourcefully and finding his way about. (Hart, 1981, p. 222)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Acredolo, L. P. (1979). Laboratory versus home: The effect of environment on the 9month-old infant’s choice of spatial reference system. Developmental Psychology, 15, 666–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P. (1982). The familiarity factor in spatial research. In R. Cohen (Ed.), Children’s conceptions of spatial relationships, (pp. 19–30 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M.D.S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1971). Individual differences in the strange situation behavior of one-year-olds. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), The origins of human social relations, (pp. 17–57 ). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anooshian, L. J., & Siegel, A. W. (1986). From cognitive to procedural mapping. In C. J. Brainard & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory development, (pp. 47–101 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arend, R., Gove, F., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Continuity of individual adaptation from infancy to kindergarten: A predictive study of ego-resiliency and curiosity in preschoolers. Child Development, 50, 950–959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. (1954). Midwest and its children: The psychological ecology of an American town. Evanston, IL: Peterson.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., Sr Most, R. K. (1980). Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: Cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analyses. Child Development, 51, 1163–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In M. Lamb Sr L. Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations, (pp. 273–309 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connell, D. B. (1974). Individual differences in infant attachment related to habituation to a redundant stimulus. Unpublished master’s thesis, Syracuse University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endsley, R. C., Hutcherson, M. A., Garner, A. P., & Martin, M. J. (1979). Interrelationships among selected maternal behaviors, authoritarianism, and preschool children’s verbal and nonverbal curiosity. Child Development, 50, 331–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, A., & Acredolo, L. P. (1979). The effect of active versus passive exploration on memory for spatial location in children. Child Development, 49, 623–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1983). Developmental levels as periods of discontinuity. In K. W. Fischer (Ed.), Levels and transitions in children’s development, (pp. 5–20 ). San Francisco: JosseyBass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., & Rogoff, B. (1986). Influence of the goal on children’s exploration and memory of large-scale space. Developmental Psychology, 22, 72–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffen, P. Sr Cole, M. (1984). Current activity for the future: The Zo-ped. In J. Wertsch & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Children’s learning in the “zone of proximal development”, (pp. 45–64 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1983). The effects of positive maternal affect on infant responses to pleasant, ambiguous, and fear-provoking toys. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit, MI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. A. (1979). Children’s experience of place. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. A. (1981). Children’s spatial representation of the landscape: Lessons and questions from a field study. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, and N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior across the life span, (pp. 195–236 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L. (1982). Spatial exploration and spatial knowledge: Individual and developmental differences in very young children. Child Development, 53, 826–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L., & Durrett, M. E. (1982). Relationship of security of attachment to exploration and cognitive mapping abilities in 2-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 18, 751–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazen, N. L., & Pick, H. L., Jr. (1985). An ecological approach to the development of spatial orientation. In T. Johnson & A. Pietrewicz (Eds.), Issues in the ecological study of learning, (pp. 201–243 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, B. B. (1984). The social context of exploratory play. In T. D. Yawkey & A. D. Pelligrini (Eds.), Child’s play: Developmental and applied, (pp. 171–201 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, C., & Endsley, R. C. (1977). The effects of a maternal model on young children’s tactual curiosity. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 131, 21–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M. D. (1984). The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 447–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klinnert, M., Campos, J., Sorce, J., Emde, R., & Svejda, M. (1983). Social referencing: Emotional expressions as behavior regulators. Emotion: Theory, research and experience (Vol. 2). Emotions in early development, (pp. 57–86 ). Orlando, FL: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leont’ev, A. N. (1981). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in Soviet psychology, (pp. 37–71 ). Armonk, NY: Sharpe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R., & Young, D. (1978). Childhood outdoors: Toward a social ecology of the landscape. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Children and the environment, (pp. 83–130 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. K. (1980). A comparison of children’s spatial reasoning: Rural Appalachia, suburban, and urban New England. Child Development, 51, 288–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pastor, D. L. (1981). The quality of mother-infant attachment and its relationship to toddler’s initial sociability with peers. Developmental Psychology, 17, 323–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Presson, C. C., & Ihrig, L. H. (1982). Using mother as a spatial landmark: Evidence against egocentric coding in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 18, 699–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1984). Introduction: Thinking and learning in social context. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context, (pp. 1–8 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adult guidance of cognitive development. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context, (pp. 95–116 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Lave, J. (Eds.). (1984). Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., & Wertsch, J. (Eds.). (1984). Children’s learning in the “zone of proximal development.”, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R. M., & Stollak, G. (1971). Curiosity and the parent-child relationship. Child Development, 42, 373–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shemyakin, F. N. (1962). Orientation in space. In B. G. Ananyev et al. (Eds.), Psychological science in the U.S.S.R., (Vol. I, pp. 186–247 ). Washington, DC: Office of Technical Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, A. W. (1981). The externalization of cognitive maps by children and adults: In search of ways to ask better questions. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behavior across the life span, (pp. 167–194 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, A. W. (1982). Toward a social ecology of cognitive mapping. In R. Cohen (Ed.), Children’s conceptions of spatial relationships, (pp. 83–94 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smirnov, A. A., & Zinchenko, P. I. (1969). Problems in the psychology of memory. In M. Cole & I. Maltzman (Eds.), A handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology, (pp. 452–502 ). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development. American Psychologist, 43, 834–841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct validation. Child Development, 50, 821–829.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, B. L., Kaban, B., Shapiro, B., & Attonucci, J. (1976). Competence and experience. In I. C. Uzgiris & F. Weizmann (Eds.), The structuring of experience, (pp. 115–152 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. H., & Siegel, A. W. (1984). Cognitive development in time and space. In B. Rogoff & J. Lave (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context, (pp. 238278 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J. (1980). Teaching the young child: Some relationships between social interaction, language, and thought. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hazen, N.L. (1989). Individual Differences in Environmental Exploration and Cognitive Mapping Skills. In: Lockman, J.J., Hazen, N.L. (eds) Action in Social Context. Perspectives in Developmental Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9000-9_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9000-9_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-9002-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-9000-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics