Theoretical Issues in Psychology pp 379-392 | Cite as
The Dynamic Unconscious Revisited: The Role of Motivation, Affect, Embodiment and Intersubjectivity in Catching Ourselves Unawares
Summary
Bringing to light a strand of Freud’s thought not acknowledged in contemporary debate establishes a single epistemic view of unconscious processes which blurs the current sharp distinction between a cognitive unconscious and a dynamic unconscious. The difference is one of degree (of affective and motivational influences) rather than a difference in kind. In the laboratory there is no question as to why material is processed at an unconscious level, because fleeting presentation or “masking” ensures its unconscious status. The question is what plays this role in real-life? Affective and motivational influences are implicated both in rendering material dynamically unconscious and in the means by which the mind grasps at its own activity, in how the habitual becomes open to the possible. These concerns bring to the fore the role of embodiment and intersubjectivity as means by which we become consciously aware, they are discussed within a psychoanalytic theory of mind with implications for a comprehensive theoretical and empirical psychology.
Keywords
Conscious Awareness Contemporary Debate Psychoanalytic Theory Unconscious Process Causal EfficacyPreview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
References
- Andersen, S. (1992). Toward a psychodynamically relevant empirical science. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 14–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Attwood, B. (1989). The making of the Aborigines. Sydney, NSW: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
- Bargh, J. A. (1992). Does subliminality matter to social psychology? In R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittam (Eds), Perception without awareness (pp. 236–255 ). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
- Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyver Jnr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition, 10 (pp. 1–61 ).Google Scholar
- Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum. Baumeister, R. F. (1992). Neglected aspects of self theory: Motivation, interpersonal aspects, culture, escape and existential value. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 21–29.Google Scholar
- Bruner, J. (1993). The autobiographical process. In Robert Folkenflik (Ed.) The Culture of autobiography: Constructions of self-representation (pp. 38–56 ). California: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
- Churchland, P. M. and Churchland, P. S. (Eds) (1998). On the contrary: Critical essays, 1987–1997. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Edelman, G. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
- Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Freud, S. (1900/1976). The interpretation of dreams. Standard edition, I, London: Hogarth. Freud, S. (1915/1984). The unconscious. Standard edition, XIV,London: Hogarth.Google Scholar
- Freud, S. (1916–17/1976). Pelican Freud Library,Volume 1. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Graham, G. (1998). Philosophy of mind: An introduction. (2nd ed.) London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
- Greenberg, M. A. (1995). Cognitive processing of traumas: the role of intrusive thoughts and reappraisals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1262–1296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Griffiths, P. E. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lear, J (1990). Love and its place in nature: A philosophical interpretation of Freudian psychoanalysis. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
- Lewicki, P. (1985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances on subsequent judgements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 463–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Maze, J. (1983). The meaning of behaviour. London: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
- Ogden, T. H. (1979). On projective identification. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 60, 357–373.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Petocz, A. (1999). Freud, psychoanalysis and symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Phillips, A. (1994). On flirtation. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Westen, D. (1992). The cognitive self and the psychoanalytic self: Can we put our selves together? Psychological Inquiry, 3, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- White, M. (1997). Narratives of therapists’ lives. Adelaide SA: Dulwich Centre Publications.Google Scholar
- Wollheim, R. (1971). Freud. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
- Woody, J.M. & Phillips, J. (1995). Freud’s “Project for a scientific psychology” after 100 years: The unconscious mind in the era of cognitive neuroscience. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology, 2, 123–134.Google Scholar