Learning via Model Construction and Criticism

Protocol Evidence on Sources of Creativity in Science
  • John Clement
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

There is growing recognition that mental models play a fundamental role in the comprehension of science concepts. The process of learning via model construction appears to be central to theory formation in science and central for science instruction but is still very poorly understood. This chapter uses evidence from case studies, in which a scientist is asked to think out loud, to argue that nonformal reasoning processes that are neither deductive nor inductive can play an important role in scientific model construction. The construction process is complex and involves repeated passes through a cycle of hypothesis generation, evaluation, and modification.

Keywords

Model Construction Explanatory Model Scientific Model Analogous Case Hypothesis Formation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Achinstein, P. (1970). Inference to scientific laws. In R. Stuewer (Ed.), Minnesota studies in philosophy of science, vol. 5: Historical and philosophical perspectives of science (pp. 87–104 ). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  2. Ackerman, R. (1965). Theories of knowledge: A critical introduction. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  3. Black, M. (1979). More about metaphor: In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (p. 19–45 ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Campbell, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67 (6), 380–400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Campbell, N. (1920). Physics: The elements. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Republished in 1957 as The foundations of science. New York: Dover. )Google Scholar
  6. Clement, J. (1981). Analogy generation in scientific problem solving. Proceedings of the Third Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 3, 137–140.Google Scholar
  7. Clement, J. (1982). Analogical reasoning patterns in expert problem solving. Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 4, 79–81.Google Scholar
  8. Clement, J. (1986). Methods for evaluating the validity of hypothesized analogies. Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 8, 223–234.Google Scholar
  9. Clement, J. (1988). Observed methods for generating analogies in scientific problem solving. Cognitive Science, 12, 563–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clement, J. (in press a). Nonformal reasoning in experts and in science students: The use of analogies, extreme cases, and physical intuition. In J. Voss, D. Perkins, & J. Siegel, Informal reasoning and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Clement, J. (in press b). Use of physical intuition in expert problem solving. In D. Tirosch & S. Straus (Eds.), Implict and explicit knowledge, an educational approach. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers.Google Scholar
  12. Clement, J. (1987, July), with the assistance of Brown, D., Camp, C., Kudukey, J., Minstrell, J., Palmer, D., Schultz, K., Shimabukuro, J., Steinberg, M., & Veneman, V. Overcoming students’ misconceptions in physics: The role of anchoring intuitions and analogical validity. Proceedings of the Second International Seminar on Misconceptions and Educational Strategies in Science and Mathematics (pp. 84–97 ). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University, Department of Education.Google Scholar
  13. Darden, L. (1982). Artificial intelligence and philosophy of science: Reasoning by analogy in theory construction. Philosophy of Science Association 2, 147–165.Google Scholar
  14. Darden, L., & Rada, R. (1988). Hypothesis formation via interrelations. In A. Prieditis (Ed.), Analogica (pp. 109–127 ). Los Altos, CA: Kaufman.Google Scholar
  15. Darwin, C. (1892/1958). In F. Darwin (Ed.), The autobiography of Charles Darwin and selected letters. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
  16. Easley, J. (1978). Symbol manipulation reexamined: An approach to bridging a chasm. In B. Presseisen, D. Goldstein, & M. Appel (Eds.), Topics in cognitive development (Vol. 2, pp. 99–112 ). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  17. Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics: An educational approach. Boston, MA: D. Reidel.Google Scholar
  18. Gentner, D. (1982). Are scientific analogies metaphors? In D. Miall (Ed.), Metaphor: Problems and perspectives (pp. 106132 ). Brighton, Sussex, England: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
  19. Getzels, J., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
  20. Gould, S. (1980). Darwin’s middle road. In S. Gould, The panda’s thumb: More reflections in natural history. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  21. Gregory, R. (1981). Mind in science. Cambridge, England. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Gruber, H. (1974). Darwin on man. New York: E. P. Dutton. Hanson, N. R. (1958). Patterns of discovery. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Havre, R. (1961). Theories and things. London, England: Newman History and Philosophy of Science Series.Google Scholar
  24. Harre, R. (1967). Philosophy of science: History of. In P. Edwards (Ed.), The encyclopedia of philosophy (pp. 289–296 ). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  25. Harre, R. (1972). The philosophies of science. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Harre, R. (1983). An introduction to the logic of the sciences. (2nd ed.) New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  27. Hesse, M. (1966). Models and analogies in science. South Bend, IN: Notre Dame University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Hesse, M. (1967). Models and analogies in science. In P. Edwards (Ed.), The encyclopedia of philosophy (pp. 354–359 ). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  29. Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Thagard, P. R. (1986). Induction: Processes of inference, learning and discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  30. Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1975). If you want to get ahead, get a theory. Cognition, 3(3), 195–212.Google Scholar
  31. Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. New York: Macmillan. Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions (1st ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  32. Kuhn, T. (1977). Concepts of cause in the development of physics. In T. Kuhn (Ed.), The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change (pp. 21–30 ). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  33. Lakatos, I. (1978). The methodology of scientific research programmes. Philosophical papers (Vol. 1 ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Langley, P. W. (1979). Rediscovering physics with BACON 3. Proceedings of the Sixth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 6, 505–507.Google Scholar
  35. Lenat, D. B. (1977). Automated theory formation in mathematics. Proceedings of the Fifth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 5, 833–842.Google Scholar
  36. Lenat, D. B., (1983). The role of heuristics in learning by discovery. In T. Mitchell (Ed.), Machine learning (pp. 243306 ). Palo Alto, CA: Tioga Publishing.Google Scholar
  37. Mansfield, R. S., & Busse, R. V. (1981). The psychology of creativity and discovery: Scientists and their work. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
  38. Nagel, E. (1961). The structure of science. New York: Harcourt, Brace, & World.Google Scholar
  39. Nersessian, N. (1984). Faraday to Einstein: Constructing meaning in scientific theories. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinas Nijhoff.Google Scholar
  40. Peirce, C. S. (1958). In C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss, & A. Burks (Eds.), Collected papers Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Perkins, D. (1981). The mind’s best work. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  43. Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
  44. Rada, R. (1985). Gradualness facilitates knowledge refinement. I.E.E.E. Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 7 (5), 523–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rothenberg, A. (1979). The emerging goddess. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  46. Schweber, S. (1977). The origin of the Origin revisited. Journal of the History of Biology, 10, 229–316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Suppe, F. (1974). The search for philosophic understanding of scientific theories. In F. Suppe (Ed.), The structure of scientific theories. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  48. Toulmin, S. (1972). Human understanding (Vol. 1 ). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Tweney, R. (1985). Faraday’s discovery of induction: A cognitive approach. In D. Goodling & F. James (Eds.), Faraday rediscovered: Essays on the life and work of Michael Faraday, 1791–1867 (pp. 189–209 ). New York: Stockton Press.Google Scholar
  50. Tweney, R. (in press). Fields of enterprise: On Michael Faraday’s thought. In D. Wallace and E. Gruber (Eds.), Creative people at work. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • John Clement
    • 1
  1. 1.Scientific Reasoning Research InstituteUniversity of MassachusettsAmherstUSA

Personalised recommendations