Skip to main content

Mechanisms and Development of Cerebral Lateralization in Children

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Critical Issues in Neuropsychology ((CINP))

Abstract

The sequences of cognitive development to be considered in this discussion culminate in an endpoint of lateralization in the mature human nervous system that in its broad definition is no longer in dispute. In the right-handed majority, language-related processes are left lateralized in almost every case. The right hemisphere does contribute toward certain aspects of verbal behavior, however (Hécaen, 1978; Searleman, 1977, 1983), notably comprehension of logical relationships, inference, metaphor, and humor (Beeman, 1993; Gardner, Ling, Flamm, & Silverman, 1975), and at the output stage, intonation, particularly when it reflects the emotional tone of the utterance (Ross & Mesulam, 1979). The left hemisphere is also specialized for rapid sequential recognition of familiar input, nonverbal as well as verbal, and also the recall and recognition of order information and the formulation of action plans, both motor and conceptual. Right hemisphere dominance is best documented for certain spatial-relational processes, particularly in the visual modality, and for the processing of emotional information (Kinsbourne, 1982). Non-right-handers deviate from the dextral norm in the following manner: in addition to left-sided representation of language, which is as prevalent in non-right-handers as in right-handers, language is also represented on the right in up to 70% of the cases (Gloning, Gloning, Haub, & Quatember, 1959). In non-right-handers, spatial-relational functions are right lateralized as in right-handers, but involve the left hemisphere also in more than half of all instances (Bryden Hécaen, & DeAgostini, 1983). Within each hemisphere the territory involved in cognitive function is more extensive in the left-than the right-hander (Kinsbourne, unpublished analysis of data from Bryden et al., 1983). Gender-related differences in lateralization are more contentious (Lewis & Harris, 1990), and the claim that language and visuospatial functions are more bilateralized in females than males, and even that left intrahemispheric organization differs between the sexes, are not yet well substantiated. With respect to development, existing knowledge is virtually restricted to language functions on the one hand and spatial-relational functions on the other. This discussion will therefore be confined to considering how peripheral laterality, notably hand preference, is established in the developing child, and how the differential hemispheric representation of language and spatial-relational functions develops, first in the normal case, and then in certain types of developmental disability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Affleck, G.,& Joyce, P. (1979). Sex differences in the association of cerebral hemisphere specialization of spatial function with conservation task performance. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 271–280.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, M. (1983). Models of hemispheric specialization. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 73–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, G., Quinn, P. T.,& Sorby, A. (1972). Stuttering: An investigation into cerebral dominance for speech. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 35, 414–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Annett, M. (1973). Handedness in families. Annals of Human Genetics, 37, 93–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. M.,& Eisele, J. A. (1994). Intellectual stability in children with unilateral brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 32(1), 85–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. M., Ekelman, B. L., Rose, D. F.,& Whitaker, H. A. (1985). Verbal and cognitive sequelae following unilateral lesions acquired in early childhood. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 55–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakan, P. (1971). Handedness and birth order. Nature, 229, 195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, D. J., Licht, R., Kok, A.,& Bouma, A. (1980). Cortical responses to word reading by right-and left-eared normal and reading-disturbed children. Neuropsychologia, 2, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, D. J., Van der Vlugt, H.,& Claushuis, M. (1978). The reliability of dichotic ear asymmetry in normal children. Neuropsychologia, 16, 753–758.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrera, M. E., Dalrymple, A.,& Witelson, S. F. (1978). Behavioral evidence of right hemisphere asymmetry in early infancy. Paper presented to the Canadian Psychological Association, Ottawa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basser, S. (1962). Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain, 85, 427–460.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., O’Connell, B., Vaid, J., Sledge, P.,& Oakes, L. (1986). Language and hand preference in early development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beeman, M. (1993). Semantic processing in the right hemisphere may contribute to drawing inferences from discourse. Brain and Language, 44, 80–120.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., Hoffman, H.,& Glanville, B. B. (1982). Development of infant ear asymmetries for speech and music. Perception and Psychophysics, 35, 75–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1988). Can the right hemisphere mediate language as well as the left? A critical review of recent research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 353–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, H.,& Legein, C. P. (1977). Fovea] and parafoveal recognition of letters by dyslexics and average readers. Neuropsychologia, 15, 69–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw-McAnulty, G., Hicks, R. E.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1984). Pathological left-handedness and familial sinistrality in relation to degree of mental retardation. Brain and Cognition, 3, 349–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg, V. (1977). The concept of symmetry in neuroanatomy. Annals of the New YorkAcademy of Sciences, 299, 186–196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braitenberg, V.,& Kemali, M. (1970). Exceptions to bilateral symmetry in the epithalamus of lower vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 138, 137–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresson, F., Maury, L., Pierant-LeBonniec, G.,& deSchonen, S. (1977). Organization and lateralization of reaching in infants: An instance of asymmetric functions in hand collaborations. Neuropsychologia, 15, 311–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P.,& Allard, F. A. (1979). Dichotic listening and the development of linguistic processes. In M. Kinsbourne (Ed.), Asymmetrical function of the brain (pp. 392–404 ). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P.,& Allard, F. A. (1981). Do auditory perceptual asymmetries develop? Cortex, 17, 313–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P., Hécaen, H.,& DeAgostini, M. (1983). Patterns of cerebral organization. Brain and Language, 20, 249–262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, P. J.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Baby drops the rattle: Asymmetry of duration grasp by infants. Child Development, 47, 532–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, P. J.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1981). Cerebral lateralization, preferred cognitive mode, and reading ability in normal children. Brain and Language, 14, 349–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, B.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Cognitive style and dichotic asymmetries of disabled readers. Cortex, 18, 357–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S.,& Diamond, R. (1977). From piecemeal to configu- rational representation of faces. Science, 195, 312–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, A., Nachson, I.,& Starinsky, R. (1976). Developmental aspects of visual hemifield differences in perception of verbal material. Brain and Language, 3, 463–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, J. G., Dooling, E. C.,& Gilles, F. H. (1977). Left-right asymmetries of the temporal speech areas of the human fetus. Archives of Neurology, 34, 346–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchill, J. A., Igna, E.,& Senf, R. (1962). The association of position at birth and handedness. Pediatrics, 29, 307–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. I. (1966). Hand preference and developmental status of infants. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 108, 337–345.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coiffi, J.,& Kandel, G. (1976). Laterality of stereognostic accuracy of children for words, shapes, and bigrams: Sex differences for bigrams. Science, 204, 1432–1434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colby, K. M.,& Parkinson, C. (1977). Handedness in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 3–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, S. G., Kaplan, B. J.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1994). Are families of children with reading difficulties at risk for immune disorders and nonrighthandedness? Cortex, 30, 281–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J.,& Fox, N. (1982). Asymmetrical brain activity discriminates between positive versus negative affective stimuli in ten month old infants. Science, 218, 1235–1236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, M.,& Kohn, B. (1975). Comprehension of syntax in infantile hemiplegics after cerebral hemidecortification: Left hemisphere superiority. Brain and Language, 2, 475–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, R., Carey, S.,& Black, K. J. (1983). Genetic influences on the development of spatial skills during early adolescence. Cognition, 13, 167–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Entus, A. K. (1977). Hemispheric asymmetry in processing of dichotically presented speech and nonspeech stimuli by infants. In S. J. Segalowitz& F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 63–73 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etaugh, C.,& Levy, R. B. (1981). Hemispheric specialization for tactile-spatial processing in preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 621–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falzi, G., Perrone, P.,& Vignolo, L. A. (1982). Right-left asymmetry in anterior speech region. Archives of Neurology, 39, 239–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Humes, M., Kaplan, E.,& Lucci-Waterhouse, L. (1984). The question of the left hemisphere dysfunction in infantile autism. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 258–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferro, J. M., Martins, I. P.,& Tavora, L. (1984). Neglect in children. Annals of Neurology, 15, 281–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flanery, R. C.,& Balling, J. D. (1979). Developmental changes in hemispheric specialization for tactile spatial ability. Developmental Psychology, 15, 364–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgays, D. G. (1953). The development of differential word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 165–168.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fried, I., Tanguay, P. E., Boder, E., Doubleday, C.,& Greensite, M. (1981). Developmental dyslexia: Electrophysiological evidence of clinical subgroup. Brain and Language, 12, 14–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gainotti, G. (1993). The middle of the right hemisphere’s contribution to the recovery of language. Journal of Disorders of Communications, 28, 227–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M. (1984). Anatomical asymmetries. In N. Geschwind& A. M. Galaburda (Eds.), Cerebral dominance: The biological foundations (pp. 11–25 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, E,& Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18(2), 222–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H., Ling, P. K., Flamm, L.,& Silverman, J. (1975). Comprehension and appreciation of humor in brain-damaged patients. Brain, 98, 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, M. (1967). The ambidextrous universe. Baltimore: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geffen, G. (1978). The development of the right ear advantage in dichotic listening with focused attention. Cortex, 14, 169–177.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geffen, G.,& Wale, J. (1979). Development of selective listening in hemispheric asymmetry. Developmental Psychology, 15, 138–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N.,& Behan, P. (1982). Left-handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 79, 5097–5100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N.,& Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: Left-right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science, 161, 186–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A.,& Ames, L. (1947). The development of handedness. Journal of General Psychology, 70, 155–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gloning, I., Gloning, K., Haub, G.,& Quatember, R. (1959). Comparison of verbal behavior in right-handed and nonrighthanded patients with anatomically verified lesion of one hemisphere. Cortex, 5, 43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Govind, C. K.,& Pearce, J. (1986). Differential reflex activity determines claw and clasper muscle asymmetry in developing lobsters. Science, 33, 354–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, W. K. (1987). Cerebral lateralization of function: From infancy through childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 376–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halverson, H. (1937). Studies of the grasping responses of early infancy. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 51, 371–392; 392–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, A. (1987). Medicine, mind and the double brain. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, X. Y. (1981). Lateralization of speech stimuli in young Down’s syndrome children. Cortex, 17, 241–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawn, P. R.,& Harris, L. J. (1983). Hand differences in grasp duration and reaching in two-and five-month-old infants. In G. Young, S. Segalowitz, C. M. Cotter,& S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Manual specialization and the developing brain. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hécaen, H. (1978). Right hemisphere contribution to language function. In P. A. Buser& A. Rougeul-Buser (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, R. E.,& Barton, A. (1975). A note on left-handedness and severity of mental retardation. Journal of General Psychology, 127, 323–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, R. E.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1978). Lateralized concomitants of human handedness. Journal of Motor Behavior, 10, 83–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M., Hampson, E., Wong, S. C. P.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1985). Effects of eye movements on the recognition and localization of dichotic stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 4, 140–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1977). Selective listening asymmetry in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 13, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1978). Ontogeny of cerebral dominance: Evidence from time-sharing asymmetry in children. Developmental Psychology, 16, 70–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1980a). Asymmetries of selective listening and attention switching in children. Developmental Psychology, 16, 70–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1980b). Asymmetry of verbal-manual time-sharing in children: A follow-up study. Neuropsychologia, 18, 151–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1995). Progress in the measurement of laterality and implications for dyslexia research. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 249–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiscock, M., Kinsbourne, M., Caplan, B.,& Swanson, J. M. (1979). Auditory attention in hyperactive children: Effects of stimulant medication on dichotic listening performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 27–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg, F. H.,& LeMay, M. (1975). Arteriographic correlates of handedness. Neurology, 25, 218–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbs, C. L.,& Hubbs, L. C. (1944). Bilateral asymmetry and bilateral variation in fishes. Paper to Michigan Academy of Arts, Sciences and Letters, 30, 229–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, P., Raichelson, R. M., Rye, D. B.,& Wainer, B. H. (1986). Hemispherectomy in infancy enlarges the distribution of motor neurons in the remaining cerebral hemisphere. Annals of Neurology, 20, 418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman, L. H. (1940). The invertebrates. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, G. W., Marshall, R., Hall, J.,& Edmonds, J. E. (1995). Learning disabilities: Neuroanatomic asymmetries. In R. J. Davidson& K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp. 617–635 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inglis, J.,& Sykes, D. H. (1967). Some sources of variation in dichotic listening performance in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 5, 480–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, D. (1975). Cerebral speech lateralization in young children. Neuropsychologia, 13, 103–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, R. K. (1966). Observations on stammering after localized cerebral injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 29, 192–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kershner, J. R.,& King, A. J. (1974). Laterality of cognitive functions in achieving hemiplegic children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1238–1284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiessling, L. S., Denckla, M. B.,& Carlton, M. (1983). Evidence for differential hemisphere function in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 727–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, 193–201.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1971). The minor cerebral hemisphere as a source of aphasic speech. Archives of Neurology, 25, 302–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1972). Eye and head turning indicate cerebral lateralization. Science, 176, 539–541.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1974). Lateral interactions in the brain. In M. Kinsbourne& W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemisphere disconnection and cerebral functions (pp. 239–259 ). Springfield, IL: Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1975). The ontogeny of cerebral dominance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263, 244–250.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1980). A model for the ontogeny of cerebral organization in non-right-handers. In J. Herron (Ed.), Neuropsychology of left handedness (pp. 177–185 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding. American Psychologist, 37, 411–420.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1986). Sinistrality and risk for immune diseases and learning disorders: A pleiotropic gene effect? Annals of Neurology, 20, 416 (abstract).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1987). Cerebral-brainstem interactions in infantile autism. In E. Schopler& G. Mesibov (Eds.), Neurobiological theories of arousal and autism. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M.,& Caplan, P. J. (1979). Children’s learning and attention problems. Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M.,& Cook, J. (1971). Generalized and lateralized effects of concurrent verbalization on a unimanual skill. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 341–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M.,& Hicks, R. E. (1978). Functional cerebral space: Amodel for overflow, transfer and interference effects in human performance: A tutorial review. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M.& Hiscock, M. (1977). Does cerebral dominance develop? In S. J. Segalowitz& F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 171–191). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M.,& Lempert, H. (1979). Does left brain lateralization of speech arise from right-biased orienting to salient percepts? Human Development, 22, 270–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, S. P.,& Rosenfield, W. D. (1980). The hemispheric specialization for linguistic and nonlinguistic tactile stimuli in third grade children. Cortex, 16, 205–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knox, C.,& Kimura, D. (1970). Cerebral processing of nonverbal sounds in boys and girls. Neuropsychologia, 8, 227–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohn, B.,& Dennis, M. (1974). Selective impairments of visuospatial abilities in infantile hemiplegics after right hemidecortication. Neuropsychologia, 12, 505–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. (1973). Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period: Some new evidence. Language Learning, 23, 63–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latimer, H. B.,& Lowrance, E. W. (1965). Bilateral asymmetry in weight and length of human bones. Anatomical Record, 152, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeMay, M. (1976). Morphological cerebral asymmetries of modern man, fossil man, and nonhuman primate. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, 349–366.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeMay, M. (1992). Left-right dissymmetry, handedness. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 13, 493–504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeMay, M.,& Culebras, A. (1972). Human brain-pathological differences in the hemispheres demonstrable by cortical arteriography. New England Journal of Medicine, 287, 168–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lempert, H.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Effect of laterality of orientation on verbal memory. Neuropsychologia, 20, 211–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lempert, H.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1985). Possible origin of speech in selective orienting. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 62–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, S. C. (1985). Developmental changes in right hemisphere involvement in face-recognition. In C. Best (Ed.), Hemisphere function and collaboration in the child (pp. 157–191 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J. (1976). A review of evidence for a genetic component in the determination of handedness. Behavor Genetics, 6, 429–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J.,& Reid, M. (1976). Variations in writing posture and cerebral organization. Science, 194, 337–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewandowski, L. L. (1982). Hemispheric asymmetry in chil- dren. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 1011–1019.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R. S.,& Harris, L. J. (1990). Handedness, sex and spatial ability. In S. Coren (Ed.), Lefthandedness: Behavioral implications and anomalies. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liederman, J. (1983). Mechanisms underlying instability in the development of the hand preference. In G. Young, S. Segalowitz, C. M. Carter,& S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Manual specialization and the developing brain. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liederman, J.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1980a). The mechanism of neonatal rightward turning bias: A sensory or motor asymmetry? Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 223–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liederman, J.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1980b). Rightward motor bias in newborns depends upon parental right-handedness. Neuropsychologia, 18, 579–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb, J. (1918). Forced movements, tropisms and animal conduct. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lokker, R.,& Morais, J. (1985). Ear differences in children at two years of age. Neuropsychologia, 23, 127–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, W. (1932). Das Rechts-Links-Problem in Tierreich and beim Menschen. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, J. (1980). Sex differences in human brain asymmetry: A critical survey. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 215–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKain, K., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S.,& Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left hemisphere function. Science, 214, 1347–1349.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeever, W. F.,& Van Deventer, A. D. (1975). Dyslexic adolescents: Evidence of impaired visual and auditory language processing associated with normal lateralization and visual responsivity. Cortex, 11, 361–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • McRae, D., Branch, D.,& Milner, B. (1968). The occipital horns and cerebral dominance. Neurology, 18, 95–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, T., Katz, L.,& Smith, M. (1974). Laterality and reading proficiency. Neuropsychologia, 12, 131–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, T.,& Rajan, P. (1975). Lateral specialization for recognition of words and faces in good and poor readers. Neuropsychologia, 13, 489–497.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mecacci, L., Sechi, E.,& Levi, G. (1983). Abnormalities of visual evoked potentials by checkerboards in children with specific reading disability. Brain and Cognition, 2, 135–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, L. K.,& Turner, S. (1973). Development of hemifield differences in word recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 172–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molfese, D. L., Freeman, R. B.,& Palmero, D. S. (1975). The ontogeny of brain lateralization for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Brain and Language, 2, 356–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagafuchi, M. (1970). Development of dichotic and monaural hearing abilities in young children. Acta Otolaryngologica, 69, 409–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naylor, H. (1980). Reading disability and lateral asymmetry: An information processing analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 531–545.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, N.,& Bandura, M. M. (1983). Effect of age at puberty on spatial ability in girls: A question of mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 19, 215–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm. F. (1971). Neural lateralization of vocal control in a passerine bird. I. Song. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 177, 229–262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obrzut, J. E., Bolick, C. A., Bryden, M. P.,& Nicholson, J. A. (1994). Age and sex-related differences in left and right hemisphere processing by learning disabled children. Neuropsychology, 8, 75–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrzut, J. E., Hynd, G. W., Obrzut, A.,& Pirozzolo, F. J. (1981). Effect of directed attention on cerebral asymmetries in normal and learning-disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 17, 118–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrzut, J. E., Hynd, G. W.,& Zellner, R. D. (1983). Attentional deficit in learning disabled children: Evidence from visual half-field asymmetries. Brain and Cognition, 2, 89–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ojemann, G. A. (1977). Asymmetric function of the thalamus. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299, 380–396.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children. London: Chapman& Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, R. D. (1964). Development of a differentiated handedness. Psychological Bulletin, 62, 257–272.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pande, B. S.,& Singh, I. (1971). One-sided dominance in the upper limbs of human fetuses as evidenced by asymmetry in muscle and bone weight. Journal of Anatomy, 109, 457–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlow, S. E.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1981). Handwriting posture and manual motor asymmetries in sinistrals. Neuropsychologia, 19, 687–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parlow, S. E., Kinsbourne, M.,& Spencer, J. (1996). Cerebral laterality in severely mentally retarded adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 299–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W.,& Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, D. M. (1977). Cerebral lateralization in young children as measured by dichotic listening and finger tapping tasks. Neuropsychologia, 15, 417–425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinsky, S. D.,& McAdam, D. W. (1980). Electroencephalographic and dichotic indices of cerebral laterality in stutterers. Brain and Language, 11, 374–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pipe, M. E. (1983). Dichotic listening performance following auditory discrimination training in Down’s syndrome and developmentally retarded children. Cortex, 19, 481–491.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, R. J., Jr.,& Berlin, C. I. (1975). On interpreting developmental changes in the dichotic right-ear advantage. Brain and Language, 2, 186–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Provine, R. R.,& Westerman, J. A. (1979). Crossing the midline: Limits of early eye-hand behavior. Child Development, 50, 437–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, D. S. (1980). Beginnings of bimanual handedness and speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 67–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, D. S. (1984). Onset of unimanual handedness in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 377–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rankin, J. M., Aram, D. M.,& Horwitz, S. J. (1981). Language ability in right and left hemiplegic children. Brain and Language, 14, 292–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinvang, I., Bakke, S. J., Hugdahl, K., Karesen, N. R.,& Sundet, K., (1994). Dichotic listening performance in relation to callosal area on the MRI scan. Neuropsychology, 8, 445–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, D. M. Q.,& Jeeves, M. A. (1978). A developmental study of hemisphere specialization for alphabetical stimuli. Cortex, 14, 259–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ringo, J. L., Doty, R. W., Demeter, S.,& Simard, P. Y. (1994). Time is of the essence: A conjecture that hemispheric specialization arises from intrahemispheric conduction delay. Cerebral Cortex, 4, 331–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. (1984). Developmental changes in hemispheric specialization for tactual processing in very young children: Evidence from cross-modal transfer. Developmental Psychology, 20, 568–574.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, E. D.,& Mesulam, M. M. (1979). Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology, 36, 144–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, G. D., Sherman, G. E.& Galaburda, A. M. (1989). Interhemispheric connection differ between symmetrical and asymmetrical brain regions. Neuroscience, 33, 525–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bartak, L.,& Newman, S. (1971). Autism: A central disorder of cognition and language? In M. Rutter (Ed.), Infantile autism: Concepts, characteristics and treatment. London: Churchill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P. (1972). Pathological lefthandedness: An explanatory model. Cortex, 8, 121–135.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P. (1976). Cerebral dominance and reading disability: An old problem revisited. In R. M. Knights& D. J. Bakker (Eds.), The neuropsychology of learning disorders. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Saslow, E.,& Henry, R. (1985). The pathological left-handedness syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 4, 27–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., Strauss, E.,& Whitaker, H. (1990). The ontogeny of hemispheric specialization: Some old hypotheses revisited. Brain and Language, 38, 596–614.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxby, L.,& Bryden, M. P. (1984). Left-ear superiority in children for processing of auditory emotional material. Developmental Psychology, 20, 72–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J.,& Maer, F. (1975). Right hemisphere lateralization for emotion in the human brain: Interactions with cognition. Science, 190, 286–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searleman, A. (1977). A review of right hemisphere linguistic capabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 503–508.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searleman, A. (1983). Language capabilities of the right hemisphere. In A. W. Young (Ed.), Functions of the right cerebral hemisphere. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searleman, A., Porac, C.,& Coren, S. (1982). The relationship between birth stress and writing hand posture. Brain and Cognition, 1, 158–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segalowitz, S. J.,& Berge, B. E. (1995). Functional asymmetries in infancy and early childhood: A review of electrophysiologic studies and their implication. In R. J. Davidson& K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp. 579–616 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segalowitz, S. J.,& Chapman, J. S. (1980). Cerebral asymmetry for speech in neonate: A behavioral measure. Brain and Language, 9, 281–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seth, G. (1973). Eye-hand coordination and “handedness”: A development study of visuomotor behaviour in infants. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 43, 35–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D. P.,& Studdert-Kennedy, M. A. (1967). A continuum of lateralization for speech perception? Brain and Language, 2, 212–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shucard, J. L., Shucard, D. W., Cummins, K. R.,& Campos, J. J. (1981). Auditory evoked potentials and sex-related differences in brain development. Brain and Language, 13, 91–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidtis, J. J., Sadler, A. E.,& Nass, R. D. (1987). Dichotic complex pitch and speech discrimination in 7 to 12 year old children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 227–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silva, D.,& Satz, P. (1984). Pathological left-handedness and ambiguous handedness: A new explanatory model. Neuropsychologia, 22, 511–515.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soper, H. V., Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Van Gorp, W. G.,& Gireer, M. F. (1987). Handedness distribution within severe to profound mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 92, 94–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiles-Davis, J., Sugarman, S.,& Nass, R. (1985). The development of spatial and class relations in four young children with right hemisphere damage: Evidence for an early spatial constructive deficit. Brain and Cognition, 4, 388–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Kershner, J. R.,& Oliver, J. (1984). Do individuals with Down’s syndrome possess right hemisphere language dominance? Cortex, 20, 221–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teng, E. L., Lee, P., Yang, K.,& Chang, P. C. (1976). Genetic, cultural and neuropathological factors in relation to laterality. In D. O. Walton, L. Rogers,& J. H. Finzi-Fried (Eds.), Conference on human brain function: Brain information service. Los Angeles: BRC Publication, University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teuber, H.-L.,& Rudel, R. G. (1962). Behavior after cerebral lesions in children and adults. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 4, 3–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Kelly, R.,& Burton, J. (1979). Hemispheric changes in information processing during development. Developmental Psychology, 14, 214–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis. L. E. (1927). Studies in stuttering. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 18, 671–690, 998–1014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman A. M.,& Geffen, G. (1968). Selective attention and cerebral dominance in perceiving and responding to speech messages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 139–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, L. Y. (1982). Handedness in autistic children and their families. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12, 421–423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turkewitz, G. (1977). The development of lateral differentiation in the human infant. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299, 309–317.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turkewitz, G.,& Ross-Kossak, P. (1984). Multiple modes of right hemisphere information processing: Age and sex differences in facial recognition. Developmental Psychology, 20, 95–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vargha-Khadem, F.,& Corballis, M. C. (1979). Cerebral asymmetry in infants. Brain and Language, 8, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vargha-Khadem, E, O’Gorman, A. M.,& Wafters, G. V. (1985). Aphasia and handedness in relation to hemispheric side, age at injury and severity of cerebral lesion during childhood. Brain, 8(3), 677–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wada, J. A., Clark, R.,& Hamm, A. (1975). Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in humans. Archives of Neurology, 32, 239–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, N.,& Kinsbourne, M. (1980). Does speech output control lateralization over time? Evidence from verbal-manual time sharing tasks. Brain and Language, 10, 215–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F. (1974). Hemispheric specialization for linguistic and nonlinguistic tactual perception using a dichotomous stimulation technique. Cortex, 10, 3–17.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F. (1976). Sex and the single hemisphere: Right hemisphere specialization for spatial processing. Science, 193, 425–427.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F.,& Kigar, D. L. (1988). Asymmetry in brain function follows asymmetry in anatomical form: Gross, microscopic, postmortem and imaging studies. In F. Boller& J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. I, pp. 114–142 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witelson, S. F.,& Pallie, W. (1973). Left hemisphere specializa- tion for language in the newborn. Brain, 96, 641–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, E, Stump, D., McKeehan, A., Sheldon, S.,& Proctor, J. (1980). Patterns of regional cerebral blood flow during attempted reading aloud by stutterers both on and off haloperidol medication: Evidence for inadequate left frontal activation during stuttering. Brain and Language, 9, 141–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woods, B. T.,& Teuber, H. L. (1978). Changing patterns of childhood aphasia. Annals of Neurology, 32, 239–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yazgan, M. Y., Wexler, B. E., Kinsbourne, M., Peterson, B.,& Leckman, J. F. (1995). Functional significance of individual variations in callosal area. Neuropsychologia, 33, 769–779.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeni-Komshian, G. H.,& Benson, D. A. (1976). Anatomical study of cerebral asymmetry in the temporal lobe of humans, chimpanzees and rhesus monkeys. Science, 192, 387–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W.,& Bion, P. J. (1980). Absence of any developmental trend in right hemisphere superiority for face recognition. Cortex, 16, 113–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. W.,& Ellis, H. D. (1976). An experimental investigation of developmental differences in ability to recognize faces presented to the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 14, 495–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, G.,& Gagnon, M. (1990). Neonatal laterality, birth stress, familial sinistrality, and left-brain inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 127–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, J. Z. (1962). Why do we have two brains? In V. B. Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. Baltimore: Johns-Hopkins Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zurif, E. B.,& Carson, G. (1970). Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance and temporal analysis. Neuropsychologia, 8, 351–361.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kinsbourne, M. (1997). Mechanisms and Development of Cerebral Lateralization in Children. In: Reynolds, C.R., Fletcher-Janzen, E. (eds) Handbook of Clinical Child Neuropsychology. Critical Issues in Neuropsychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-5351-6_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-5351-6_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-5353-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-5351-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics