The Cognitive Architecture of Risk: Pancultural Unity or Cultural Shaping?

  • Eugene A. Rosa
  • Noriyuki Matsuda
  • Randall R. Kleinhesselink
Chapter
Part of the Technology, Risk, and Society book series (RISKGOSO, volume 13)

Summary

The intention of this study has been to test the hypothesis of “universalism of risk perception” using several student samples from the United States and Japan. The main objective was to reveal hidden patterns of risk perception based on psychometric characteristics among and between the different samples. In particular, the study was designed to demarcate between pancultural and culturally variant features of the cognitive processes of risks. One key element was the careful selection of a comparative culture. A comparison between Japan and a Western culture provides one of the most stringent tests of the hypotheses guiding the research program. Another key element was the development of a comparative instrument, in Phase I, which met a number of language translation criteria and tests of face validity.

Phase II of the research program produced a rich data set with which to test, not only the specified hypothesis, but also to perform sub-analyses and other refinements. This data set was first analyzed using the standard factor-analytic techniques of the standard psychometric paradigm. In general, the findings from the United States-Japan comparison replicated previous results produced within this paradigm — including the cross-cultural results. The cognitive maps produced by this analysis indicated common contours between the Americans and Japanese, but different cognitive content. Without a formal basis for judging whether contour or content should be the criterion for determining which of our hypotheses were supported, the fundamental questions generating the research program remained unresolved.

To resolve them we turned to latent variable modeling in Phase III of the research program, still in progress. Thus far this phase has produced preliminary results on only the sub-set of seven nuclear risks contained in the data set. These results are, nevertheless, promising. We developed a refined, best-fitting model for each culture based upon the nuclear items. What these models seem to be telling us is that Americans and Japanese have similar images of dread and catastrophic potential underlying their perceptions of nuclear risks. But, importantly, the cognitive routes they use to arrive at those images are fundamentally different.

Keywords

Risk Perception Nuclear Weapon Latent Variable Model Cognitive Architecture Japanese Sample 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bandura, A. 1986. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  2. Bentler, P. 1989. EQS: Structural Equations Program Manual. Los Angeles, CA: BMDP Statistical Software, Inc.Google Scholar
  3. Boholm, A. 1996. The Many Faces of Cultural Theory of Risk: Anthropological Misgivings. Paper presented at the annual meetings of the Society for Risk Analysis-Europe, Guildford, University of Surrey, UK.Google Scholar
  4. Boholm, A. 1998. Comparative Studies of Risk Perception: A Review of Twenty Years of Research. Journal of Risk Research 1: 135–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brislin, R. 1993. Understanding Culture’s Influence on Behavior. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Publishers.Google Scholar
  6. Byrne, B. M. 1994. Structural Equation Modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  7. Covello, V. T. and Kawamura, K. 1988. Cooperation vs. confrontation: A comparison of approaches to environmental risk management in Japan and the United States. Risk Analysis 8: 161–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Covello, V. T., von Winterfeldt, D. and Slovic, R. 1986. Risk communication: A review of the literature. Risk Abstracts 3: 71–182.Google Scholar
  9. Cvetkovich, C. and Earle, T. C. 1985. Classifying hazardous events. Journal of Environmental Psychology 13: 5–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cvetkovich, G., Vlek, C. and Earle, T. C. 1988. Designing public hazard communication programs about large-scale technologies, in C. Vlek and G. Cvetkovich (Eds.), Social Decision Methods for Large-Scale Technologies. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
  11. Deaux, K. and Wrightsman, L. S. 1988. Social Psychology, 5th edition. Pacific Grove: Brooks/ Cole.Google Scholar
  12. Douglas, M. and Wildaysky, A. 1982. Risk and Culture. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  13. Englander,T., Farago, K., Slovic, P, and Fischhoff, B. 1986. A comparative analysis of risk perception in Hungary and the United States. Social Behaviour 1: 55–66.Google Scholar
  14. Form, W. 1979. Comparative industrial sociology and the convergence hypothesis. Annual Review of Sociology 5: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Frane, J. W. and Hill, M. 1976. Factor analysis as a tool for data analysis. Communication Statistics A5 (6): 487–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Goszczynzka, M., Tyszka, T. and Slovic, R. 1991. Risk perception in Poland: A comparison with three other countries. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 4: 179–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Heisenberg, W. 1958. The Physicist’s Conception of Nature. Trans. by Arnold J. Pomrtrans. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  18. Hinman, G. W., Rosa, E. A., Kleinhesselink, R.R. and Lowinger, T.C. 1993. Perceptions of Nuclear and Other Risks in Japan and the United States. Risk Analysis 13: 449–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hoshino, K. 1989. Personal communication, National Institute of Police Science, Tokyo, Japan.Google Scholar
  20. Johnson, E. J. and Tversky, A. 1984. Dimensional analysis of risk perceptions. Journal of Exuerimental Psychology: General 113: 53–64.Google Scholar
  21. Kahneman, D., Slovic, P and Tversky, A. (Eds.) 1982. Judgment Under Uncertainity: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Karpowicz-Lazreg, C. and Mullet, E. 1993. Societal risk as seen by the French public. Risk Analysis 13: 253–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Keown, C. F. (1989). Risk perceptions of Hong Kongese vs. Americans. Risk Analysis 9: 401–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kleinhesselink, R. R. and Rosa, E. A. 1994. Nuclear Trees in a Forest of Hazards: A Comparison of Risk Perceptions between American and Japanese University Students. Pp. 101–119. in Thomas C. Lowinger and George W. Hinman (eds.), Nuclear Power at the Crossroads: Challenges and Prospects for the Twenty-First Century. Boulder, CO: International Research Center for Energy and Economic Development, University of Colorado.Google Scholar
  25. Kleinhesselink, R. R. and Rosa, E. A. 1991. Cognitive Representation of Risk Perceptions: A Comparison of Japan and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 22: 11–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kraus, N.H. and Slovic, P. 1988. Taxonomic Analysis of Perceived Risk: Modeling Individual and Group Perceptions Within Homogeneous Hazard Domains. Risk Analysis 8: 435–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M. and Combs, B. 1978. Judged frequency of lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 4: 551–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Loehlin, J. C. 1992. Latent Variable Models: An Introduction to Factor, Path, and Structural Analysis. (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  29. Mazur, A. 1982. The Dynamics of Technical Controversy. Washington DC: Communications Press.Google Scholar
  30. National Research Council. 1989. Improving Risk Communication. Washington DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
  31. Nyland, L. G. 1993 Risk Perception in Brazil and Sweden. Rhizikon Risk Research Reports No. 15, Stockholm: Center for Risk Research, Stockholm School of Economics.Google Scholar
  32. Renn, O. and Swaton, E. 1984. Psychological and Sociological Approaches to the Study of Risk Perception. Environment International 10: 557–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rosa, E. A. 1998. Metatheoretical Foundations for Post-Normal Risk. Journal of Risk Research 1: 15–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rosa, E. A. and Kleinhesselink, R.R. 1990. Risk Perceptions in Japan and the United States: An Empirical Test of Cultural Influences. Paper presented at the annual meetings of the Pacific Sociological Association, Spokane, WA, USA (April).Google Scholar
  35. Rosa, E.A. and Matsuda, N. 1997. The Cognitive Architecture of Risk Perceptions: Pancultural Unity or Cultural Variation? Paper presented at the annual meetings of the Society of Risk Analysis, Washington, DC, USA (December).Google Scholar
  36. Sears, D. O. 1986. College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51: 515–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sjöberg, L. 1995. Explaining risk Perception: an Empirical and Quantitative Evaluation of Cultural Theory. Rhizikon Risk Research Reports No. 22, Stockholm: Center for Risk Research, Stockholm School of Economics.Google Scholar
  38. Slovic, P. 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236: 280–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Slovic, P. 1986. Informing and educating the public about risk. Risk Analysis 6: 403–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B. and Lichtenstein, S., 1985. Characterizing perceived risk, Pp. 91–125. in R. Kates, C. Hohenemser, and R. Kasperson (Eds.), Perilous Progress: Managing the Hazards of Technology. Boulder CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  41. Teigen, K. II., Brun, W. and Slovic, P. 1988. Societal risks as seen by a Norwegian public. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 1: 111–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Thompson, M., Ellis, R. and Wildaysky, A. 1990. Cultural Theory. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eugene A. Rosa
    • 1
  • Noriyuki Matsuda
    • 2
  • Randall R. Kleinhesselink
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of SociologyWashington State UniversityPullmanUSA
  2. 2.Institute of Policy and Planning SciencesUniversity of TsukubaTsukubaJapan
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyWashington State UniversityVancouverUSA

Personalised recommendations