Skip to main content

Prosodic Development in Normal and Autistic Children

  • Chapter
Communication Problems in Autism

Part of the book series: Current Issues in Autism ((CIAM))

Abstract

Deficits in prosody have been consistently described as an integral part of the speech and language disorder in autistic children (Kanner, 1946; Ornitz & Ritvo, 1976). Such deficits still remain evident in the language characteristics of children whose speech showed considerable improvement over time (DeMyer, Barton, DeMyer, Norton, Allen, and Steele, 1973; Rutter & Lockyer, 1967; Baltaxe & Simmons, 1983). However, there is still a paucity of research investigating the deficits in this important aspect of speech and language. The present review is an effort to summarize prosodic studies in autism and to consider the findings to date in terms of what is known about prosody in normal language acquisition and in language pathology. Finally, we will present some speculation on how the prosodic deficits might fit into the general picture of the autistic language disturbance and brain dysfunction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allen, G., & Hawkins, S. (1980). Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. In G. YeniKomshian, J. Kavanagh, and C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (pp. 227–256 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Applebaum, E., Egel, A., Koegel, R., & Imhoff, B. (1979). Measuring musical abilities of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 279–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, G., & Schwartz, S. (1983). Hemispheric lateralization of language in autistic and aphasic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, 129–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson-King, K. (1973). Children’s acquisition of phonological stress contrasts. Working Papers in Phonetics, Vol. 21. Los Angeles: University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bagshaw, N. (1978). An acoustic analysis of fundamental frequency and temporal parameters of autistic children’s speech. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C. (1981). Acoustic characteristics of prosody in autism. International Congress for the Scientific Study of Mental Deficiency. In P. Mittler (Ed.), New frontiers of knowledge in the scientific study of mental deficiency, (pp. 223–233 ). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C. (1984). The use of contrastive stress in normal, aphasic and autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 97–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Guthrie, D. (in press). The acquisition of primary sentence stress in normal, aphasic and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,(in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1973). Echolalia and language acquisition in autism. Paper presented at the American Speech and Hearing Association, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1975). Language in childhood psychosis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 40, 439–458.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1977). Language patterns of German and English autistic adolescents. In P. Mittler (Ed.), Proceedings of the International Association for the Study of Mental Deficiency (pp. 223–233 ). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1981). Disorders of language in childhood psychosis: Current concepts and approaches. In J. Darby (Ed.), Speech evaluations in psychiatry, (pp. 285–325 ). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., & Simmons, J. Q. (1983). Communication deficits in the adolescent and adult autistic. Seminars in Speech and Language, 4, 27–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C., Simmons, J. Q., & Zee, E. (1984). Intonation patterns in normal, autistic and aphasic children. In A. Cohen and M. van de Broecke (Eds.), Proceedings of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 713–718 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1979). A functionalist approach to the acquisition of grammar. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Development pragmatics (pp. 167–211 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, S., Kelsch, Daniloff, J., Daniloff, R., & Lewis, J. (1982). Sentence comprehension in Broca’s aphasics: Effects of some supresegmental variables. Brain and Language, 17, 261–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackstock, E. (1978). Cerebral asymmetry and the development of infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 339–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blasdell, R., & Jensen, P. (1970). Stress and word position as determinates of imitation in first-language learners. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 193–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. L. (1964). Around the edge of language. Harvard Educational Review, 34, 282–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonvillian, J. D., Raeburn, V. P., & Horan, E. A. (1979). Talking to children: The effects of rate, intonation and length on children’s sentence imitation. Journal of Child Language, 6, 459–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, G. (1979). Some reasons why successive single word utterances are not. Journal of Child Language, 6, 411–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J. Q. (1972). Stress and syntax: A reply. Language, 48, 326–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broen, P. (1972). The verbal environment of the language learning child. American Speech and Hearing Association Monograph, Vol. 17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Bellugi, R. (1964). Three processes in the child’s acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review, 54, 133–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R., & Fraser, D. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In C. Cofer and B. Musgrave (Eds.), Verbal behavior and learning (pp. 158–197 ). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1980). Some constraints on models of language production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production, Vol. 1. Speech and talk (pp. 432–459 ). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buxton, H. (1983). Auditory lateralization, an effect of rhythm. Brain and Language, 18, 249–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound patterns of English. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., Halle, M., & Lukoff, F. (1956). On accent and juncture in English. In M. Halle (Ed.), For Roman Jacobson (pp. 65–80 ). The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1971). Dysprosody due to posterior fossa lesions. Transactions of the American Neurological Association, Vol. 96, 151–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W., & Sorensen, J. (1977). Fundamental frequency contours at syntactic boundaries. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 62, 683–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W., and Sorensen, J. (1981). Fundamental frequency in sentence production. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (1973). Non-sequential phonology in language acquisition: A review of the issues. Lingua, 32, 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (1979). Prosodic development. In P. Fletcher and M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition, (pp. 33–48 ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A. (1983). Semantics, syntax, and sentence accent. Abstracts of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 85–91 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Foss, D. J. (1977). On the role of sentence stress in sentence processing. Language and Speech, 20, 1–10.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Isard, S. D. (1980). The production of prosody. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production, Vol. 1. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., & Swinney, P. A. (1980). Development of the comprehension of semantic focus in young children. Paper presented at the Fifth Boston University Conference on language Development, Boston, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A., & Damasio, H. (1977). Musical faculty and cerebral dominance. In M. Critchley and R. A. Henson (Eds.), Music and the brain (pp. 141–154 ). London: W. Heinemann Medical Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H., Maurer, R. G., Damasio, A. R., & Chiu, H. C. (1980). Computerized tomographic scan findings in patients with autistic behavior. Archives of Neurology, 37, 504–510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danly, M., & Shapiro, B. (1982). Speech prosody in Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 16, 171–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danly, M., deVilliers, J. G., & Cooper, W. G. (1979). The control of speech prosody in Broca’s aphasia. In J. J. Wolf and D. H. Klatt (Eds.), Proceedings from the 97th Annual Meeting of the Acoustic Society of America (pp. 259–263 ). New York: Acoustic Society of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danly, M., Cooper, W. E., and Shapiro, E. (1983). Fundamental frequency processing and linguistic structure in Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 19, 1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. J. On the dynamic use of prosody in speech perception. In A. Cohen & S. G. Note-boom (Eds.). Structure and process in speech perception (pp. 178–194). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Warrenburg, S., & Fuller, P. (1982). Cerebral lateralization of individuals diagnosed as autistic in early childhood. Brain and Language, 15, 353–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeMeyer, M., Barton, S., DeMeyer, W. E., Norton, J. A., Allen. J., and Steele, R. (1973). Prognosis in autism: A follow-up study. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia 3. 199–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSimoni, F. G. (1974a). Influence of sound environment on the duration of consonants in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year-old children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 56, 360–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSimoni, F. G. (1974b). Influence of consonant environment on the duration of vowels in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year-old children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 56, 362–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiSimoni, F. G. (1974c). Some preliminary observations on temporal compensation in the speech of children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 56, 697–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language, 2, 21–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eilers, R. Suprasegmental and grammatical control over telegraphic speech in young children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 227–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin-Tripp, S. (1966). Imitation and structural change in children’s language. In E. Lenneberg (Ed.), New directions in the study of Language (pp. 163–189 ). Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks, G. (1942). An acoustic study of the pitch of infant hunger wails. Child Development, 13, 227–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks, G., Herbert, R. L., & Hammond, J. M. (1949). An acoustical study of vocal pitch in seven-and eight-year-old girls. Child Development, 20, 71–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks, G., Wiley, J. H., & Lassman, F. M. (1949). An acoustical study of focal pitch in seven-and eight-year-old boys. Child Development, 20, 63–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flege, J. E. (1982). Timing in the speech of children and adults. Paper presented at the 103rd Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Chicago, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, C. (1976). A comparison of pitch patterns of normal and autistic children. Unpublished master’s thesis. University of California at Santa Barbara.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V. (1983). The independence and dependence of syntax, semantics, and prosody. In A. Cohen & M. van de Broecke (Eds.), Abstracts of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 93–97 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry, H. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of america, 27, 765–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fry, H. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech, 1, 126–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garding, E. (1982). Prosodic expression and pragmatic categories. Working Papers, Department of Linguistics and Phonetics. Lund University, Sweden, 22, 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garding, E. (1983). Intonation units and pivots, syntax and semantics. In Abstracts of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 99–103 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnica, O. V. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow and C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 63–88 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gates, A., & Bradshaw, J. (1977). The role of cerebral hemispheres in music. Brain and Language, 4, 403–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., & Svendsen, P. (1983). Childhood psychosis and computed tomographic brain scan findings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, 19–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C. Rosenhall. U., & Johanson, E. (1983). Auditory brainstem responses in childhood psychosis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, 181–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldfarb, W., Braunstein, P., & Lorge, I. (1956). A study of speech patterns in a group of schizophrenic children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 26, 544–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldfarb, W., Goldfarb, N., Braunstein, P., & Scholl, H. (1972). Speech and language faults of schizophrenic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 219–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H. (1973). Studies on the grammar of aphasics. In H. Goodglass & S. Blumstein (Eds.), Psycholinguistics and aphasia (pp. 185–215 ). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., Fodor, I., & Schulhoff, C. (1967). Prosodic factors in grammar-evidence from aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 10, 5–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, M., & Parkinson, A. (1971). Stimulus task factors as determinants of ear advantages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 168–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, S., DeLong, G., & Rosman, M. (1970). Pneumoencephalographic findings in the infantile autism syndrome. Brain, 98, 667–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Scholes, R., & Watson, R. T. (1975). Auditory affective agnosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 38, 69–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, J. (1972). Language disabilities of emotionally disturbed children. In J. Irwin & M. Marge (Eds.). Principles of childhood language disabilities (pp. 137–158 ). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hier, D., LeMay, N., & Rosenberger, P. (1979). Autism and unfavorable left-right asymmetries of the brain. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9. 137–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hornby, P., & Haas, W. (1970). Use of contastive stress by preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 395–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huggins, A. W. (1972). On the perception of temporal phenomena in speech. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 51, 1279–1290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, and phonological universals. In A. R. Keiler (Trans.), Kin-dersprache, Aphasie and allgemeine Lautgesetze The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, A. L., & Barry, R. J. (1983). Developmental effects in the cerebral lateralization of autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, 43–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joanette, Y., Lecours, A. R., Lepage, Y., & Lamoreux, M. Language in right-handers with right-hemisphere lesions: A preliminary study including anatomical, genetic, and social factors. Brain and Language, 20, 217–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner. L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1971). Follow-up study of eleven autistic children, originally reported in 1943. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 119–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner. L. & Eisenberg, L. (1956). Early infantile autism. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 26, 556–564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E. (1970). Intonation and child language acquisition. Papers in Research in Child Language Development, 1, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, P., & Buhr, R. (1978). Fundamental frequency in the speech of infants and children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 63, 567–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellar, K., & Bever, T. (1980). Hemispheric asymmetries in the perception of musical cords as a function of musical experience and family handedness. Brain and Language, 10, 24–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, R. D., & Forner, L. L. (1980). Speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 157–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, L. (1973). Analysis of speech imitations by Gd children. Anthropological Linguistics, 15. 267–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D. G. (1975). Vowel lengthening is syntactically determined in connected discourse. Journal of Phonetics, 3, 129–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, D. (1976). Linguistic uses of sequential duration in English: Acoustic and perceptual evidence. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 59, 1208–1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In J. Lyons and R. J. Wales (Eds.), Psycholinguistic papers (pp. 183–208 ). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozhevnikov, V. A., & Chistovich, L. A. (1965). Speech: Articulation and perception, Vol. 30 (p. 543 ). Washington, DC: Joint Publication Research Service, Department of Commerce.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubaska, C. A., & Keating, P. A. (1981). Word duration in early child speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 615–621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutik, E., Cooper, W., & Boyce, S. (1983). Declination of fundamental frequency in speakers’ production of parenthetical and main clauses. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 73, 1731–1738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd. D. R. (1983). Phonological features of intonational peaks. Language, 59, 721–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jefress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms of behavior (pp. 112–136 ). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1972). The timing of utterances and linguistic boundaries. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 51, 2018–2024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1973a). Phonetic disambiguation of syntactic ambiguity. Glossa, 7, 107–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1973b). Rhythmic units and syntactic units in production and perception. Journal of Acoustic Society of American, 54, 1228–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1977). Isochrony reconsidered. Journal of Phonetics, 5, 252–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1978). Temporal organization and prosody-perceptual aspects. Paper presented at the Joint Meeting, Acoustic Society of America and Japan. Honolulu, Hawaii.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1982). The role of prosody in the internal structuring of a sentence. Preprints of the Plenary Session Papers of the Thirteenth International Congress of Linguistics, Tokyo (pp. 189–198 ).

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1983a). Signaling of syntactic structure in whipsered speech. Folia Linguistica, 7, 239–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I. (1983b). Semantics, syntax, and prosody. In Abstracts of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 69–74 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeHiste, I., Olive, J. P., & Streeter, L. A. (1976). Role of duration in disambiguating syntactically ambiguous sentences. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 60, 1199–1202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundation of language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, M. Y. (1983). In favor of some uncommon approaches to the study of speech. In P. F.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 265–274). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1968). Intonation, perception and language. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1975). On the origin of language. New York: MacMillan Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1970). A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis: IV. Patterns of cognitive ability. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 151–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, R. (1970). Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77, 211–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. (1972). Rhythmic vs. serial structure in speech and other behavior. Psychological Review, 79, 487–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. (1975). Rhythmic expectancy in continuous speech perception. In A. Cohen & S. Nooteboom (Eds.), Structure and process in speech perception (pp. 161–177 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. (1979). Rhythmic and segmental perception are not independent. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 65, 1286–1297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, Ph. (1977). A theory of English intonation. R. A. Institut de Phonetique Bruxelles, 11, 83–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, Ph. (1982). Phonetic realization of prosodic contours in French. Speech Communication, 1, 3–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, Ph. (1983). Semantics, syntax, and intonation. In Abstracts of the Tenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 75–83 ). Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, R. G., & Damasio, A. R. (1982). Childhood autism from the point of view of behavioral neurology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12, 195–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, B. S. Hemispheric asymmetries and early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 401–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menn, L. (1976). Pattern, control, and contrast in beginning speech: A case study in the development of word form and word function. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, W., & Erwin, S. (1964). The development of grammar in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29, 9–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monrad-Krohn, G. (1947). Dysprosody or altered “melody of language.” Brain, 70, 405–415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monrad-Krohn, G. (1957). The third element of speech: Prosody in the neuropsychiatric clinic. Journal of Mental Science, 103, 326–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monrad-Krohn, G. (1963). The third element of speech and its disorders. In L. Halpern (Ed.), Problems of dynamic neurology (pp. 101–118 ). Jerusalem: Rothschild Hadassah University Hospital and Hebrew University Hadassah Medical School.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse, P. (1974). Infant speech production: A preliminary model and review of the literature. In Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 477–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morese, P. (1974). Infant speech production: A preliminary model and review of the literature. In R. Schiefelbusch and L. Lloyd (Eds.), Language perspectives-acquisition, retardation and intervention (pp. 19–54 ). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murry, T., Amundson, P., and Hollien, H. (1977). Acoustic characteristics of infant cries. Journal of Child Language, 4, 321–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakatani, L., & Schaffer, J. (1978). Hearing “words” without words. Prosodic cues for word perception. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 63, 234–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hala, J. (1977). The physiology of stress. In L. M. Hyman (Ed.), Studies in stress and accent. Los Angeles: Southern California Occasional Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Hala, J. (1978). Production of tone. In Tone: A linguistic survey New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Omitz, E., & Ritvo, E. (1976). Medical assessment. In E. Ritvo (Ed.), Autism diagnosis, current research and management (pp. 7–26 ). New York: Spectrum Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Shaughnessy, D. (1979). Linguistic features in fundamental frequency patterns. Journal of Phonetics, 7, 119–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paccia, J., & Curcio, F. (1982). Language processing and forms of immediate echolalia in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 42–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Padilla, A. M., & Lindbloom, K. (1976). Development of interrogative, negative, and possessive forms in the speech of young Spanish-English bilinguals. The Bilingual Review/La Revisa Bilingua, 3, 122–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, G. E., & Barney, H. L. (1952). Control methods used in a study of the vowels. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 24, 175–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, G. E., & LeHiste, I. (1960). Duration of syllabic nuclei in English. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 32, 693–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierrehumbert, J. (1979). The perception of fundamental frequency declination. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 66, 363–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., & Bradshaw, J. (1979). Hemispheric functioning in autistic children. Cortex, 15, 73–81.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prizant, B. (1983). Gestalt language and gestalt processing in autism. Topics in language Disorders, 3, 16–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronovost, W., Wakstein, M., & Wakstein, P. (1966). A longitudinal study of the speech behavior and language comprehension of fourteen children diagnosed atypical or autistic. Exceptional Children, 33, 19–26.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rimland, B. (1964). Infantile autism. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rimland, B. (1978). Inside the mind of the autistic savant. Psychology Today, 12, 68–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, E. D., & Mesulam, M. M. (1979). Dominant language function of the right hemisphere. Prosody and emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology, 59, 721–759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1966). Prognosis: Psychotic children in adolescence and early adult life. In J. K. Wing (Ed.), Early childhood autism: Clinical, educational, and social aspects (pp. 83–100 ). London: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis. I: Description of sample. British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 1169–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Safer, M., and Leventhal, H. (1977). Ear differences in evaluating emotional tones of voice and verbal content. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 3, 75–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1979). Non-linguistic vocal indicators of emotions and psychopathology. In C. E. Isard (Ed.), Emotions in personality and psychopathology (pp. 493–529 ). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1981a) Speech and emotional states. In J. Darby (Ed.), Speech evaluation in psychiatry (pp. 189–220 ). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1981b). Vocal indicators of stress and speech production. In J. Darby (Ed.), Speech evaluation in psychiatry (pp. 171–187 ). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlanger, B., Schlanger, P., & Gerstman, L. (1976). The perception of emotionally toned sentences by right hemisphere damaged and aphasic subjects. Brain and Language, 3, 396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seashore, C., Lewis, D., & Saetit, J. G. (1956). Manual for the Seashore Test of Musical Talents (rev. ed.). New York: The Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlackova, E., & Neshidalova, R. (1975). Development of autistic children with special regard to their means of verbal expression. Folia Phoniatrica, 27, 157–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard, W. C., & Lane, H. L. (1968). Development of the prosodic features of infant vocalizing. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 11, 94–108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwin, A. (1953). Reactions to music of autistic (schizophrenic) children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 823–831.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. Q., & Baltaxe, C. (1975). Language patterns in adolescent autistics. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 333–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, N. (1975). Echolalic speech in childhood autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 1439–1446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. (1978). Temporal aspects of English speech production: A developmental prospective. Journal of Phonetics, 6, 37–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, B. K., Sugarman, M., & Long, S. (1983). Experimental manipulation of speaking rate for studying temporal variabilities in children’s speech. Journal of the Acoustic Society of American, 74, 744–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring, D., & Dale, P. (1977). The discrimination of stress in early infancy. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 224–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R., & Tallal, P. (1979). Analysis of stop consonant production errors in developmentally dysphasic children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 66, 1703–1712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffenson, M. S. (1977). Satisfying inquisitive adults: Some simple methods of answering yes/no questions. Journal of Child Language, 5. 221–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swinney, D., Zurif, E., & Cutler, A. (1980). Effects of sentential stress and word class upon comprehension on Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language, 10, 132–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanguay, P. (1976). Clinical and eiectrophysiological research. In E. R. Ritvo, B.1. Freeman, E. M. Ornitz, and P. E. Tanguay (Eds.), Autism: Diagnosis, current research and management (pp. 75–84 ). New York: Spectrum Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tingley, B., & Allen, G. (1975). Development of speech timing control in children. Child Development, 46, 186–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tonkava-Yampolskaya, R. V. (1969). Development of speech intonation during the first two years of life. Soviet Psychology, 7, 48–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tonkava-Yampolskaya, R. V. (1973). Development of speech intonation in infants during the first two years of life. In C. Ferguson and D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 128–138 ). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyack, D., & Ingram, D. (1977). Children’s production and comprehension of questions. Journal of Child Language, 4, 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tymchuk, A., Simmons, J. Q., & Neafsey, S. (1971). Intellectual characteristics of adolescent childhood psychotics with high verbal ability. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 21, 133–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viscott, D. (1970). A musical idiot savant. Psychiatry, 33, 494–515.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, S., Mesulam, M., & Kramer, L. (1981). Disturbances in prosody: A right hemisphere contribution to language. Archives of Neurology, 38, 742–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weir, R. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weitz. S. (1979). Paralanguage. In S. Weitz (Ed.), Nonverbal communication: Readings with commentary (pp. 221–231 ). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wieman, L. (1976). Stress patterns in early child language. Journal of Child Language, 3, 282–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. E., & Stevens, K. N. (1981). Vocal correlates of emotional states. In J. Darby (Ed.), Speech evaluation in psychiatry (pp. 221–240 ). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Baltaxe, C.A.M., Simmons, J.Q. (1985). Prosodic Development in Normal and Autistic Children. In: Schopler, E., Mesibov, G.B. (eds) Communication Problems in Autism. Current Issues in Autism. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-4806-2_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-4806-2_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-3203-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-4806-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics