Skip to main content

Creativity and Reinforced Variability

  • Chapter

Abstract

Campbell (1960) argued that random variations are essential for creativity because, if an act is truly novel or original, it cannot be anticipated or preformulated. A random process implies, according to Campbell, that generation of variations cannot be accounted for by knowledge of prior environmental events. (Ultimately the environment selects from among the variations [see Donahoe, this volume], but Campbell’s focus, and that of the present paper, is on the variation process itself.) Campbell was convincing that variability is necessary for creativity—replication is not creative—but contemporary research shows that the process by which variations are generated is in fact influenced by the environment. Variable versus repetitive actions, levels of variability, the set of possible variants, and the probability of unlikely combinations; all of these are directly affected by consequences. Stated simply, behavioral variability can be reinforced. To the extent that variability is necessary, creativity may wither in the absence of environmental support. This claim is both controversial, because many believe that reinforcement is detrimental to creativity, a claim to which I shall return, and important, because it indicates a direction for educational and social policy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983). The Social psychology of Creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Balsam, P. D., Deich, J. D., Ohyama, T., & Stokes, P. D. (1998). Origins of new behavior. In W. O’Donohue (Ed.) Learning and Behavior Therapy (pp. 403–420 ). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balsam, P. D., Paterniti, A. Zechowy, K., & Stokes, P. D. (2002). Outcomes and behavioral variability: Disappointment induced variation. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought & action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blough, D. S. (1966). The reinforcement of least frequent interresponse times. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 581–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger, P. (1997). Variables that influence the generation of random sequences: An update. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 627 - 661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, D. W. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 1–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 64, 363–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cherot, C., Jones, A., & Neuringer, A. (1996). Reinforced variability decreases with approach to reinforcers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 497–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Verbal Behavior. By B. F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denney, J., & Neuringer, A. (1998). Behavioral variability is controlled by discriminative stimuli. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 154–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberger, R., & Armeli, S. (1997). Can salient reward increase creative performance without reducing intrinsic creative interest? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 652–663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty, C. F. (1996). Incentive relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flora, S. R. (1990). Undermining intrinsic interest from the standpoint of a behaviorist. The Psychological Record, 40, 323–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, E. M., & Baer, D. M. (1973). Social control of form diversity and emergence of new forms in children’s block building. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 209–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heidelberger, M. (1987). Fechner’s indeterminism: from freedom to laws of chance. In Kruger, L. et. al (Eds.). The Probabilistic Revolution, Vol. 1: Ideas in History (pp. 117–156 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hermstein, R. J. (1997). (H. Rachlin & D. I. Laibson, Eds.). The Matching Law. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knuth, D. E. (1969). The art of computer programming. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machado, A. (1989). Operant conditioning of behavioral variability using a percentile reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 155–166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maltzman, I. (1960). On the training of originality. Psychological Review, 67, 229–242.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mook, D. M., Jeffrey, J., & Neuringer, A. (1993). Spontaneously Hypertensive Rats (SHR) readily learn to vary but not repeat instrumental responses. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 59, 126–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moxley, R. A. (1997). Skinner: from determinism to random variation. Behavior and Philosophy, 25, 3–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. D. (1978). Fixed-interval matching-to-sample: Intermatching time and intermatching error runs. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 105–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A. (1967). Effects of reinforcement scheduling on simultaneous discrimination performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 251–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (1986). Can people behave “randomly”?: The role of feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 62–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (1991). Operant variability and repetition as functions of interresponse time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (1992). Choosing to vary and repeat. Psychological Science, 3, 246–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (2002). Operant Variability: Evidence, functions, and theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 672–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A., Kornell, N., & Olufs, M. (2001). Stability and variability in extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior processes, 27, 79–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Notterman, J. M., & Mintz, D. E. (1965). Dynamics of response. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Page, S., & Neuringer, A. (1985). Variability is an operant. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 429–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porac, J. F., & Salancik, G. R. (1981). Generic overjustification: The interaction of extrinsic rewards. Organization Behavior and Human Performance, 27, 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, K. W., Haag, R., & O’Reilly, J. (1969). The creative porpoise: Training for novel behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 653–661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (1982). Reinforcement-induced stereotypy: How not to teach people to discover rules Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 23–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1961). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. In B. F. Skinner, Cumulative Record (pp. 347–366). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts (Reprinted from Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65, 1935 ).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1968). The Technology of Teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1976). About behaviorism. NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1980). Notebooks. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1983). A Matter of Consequences. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F., & Vaughan, M. E. (1983). Enjoy Old Age: A Program of Self-Management. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1983). Adaptive Behavior and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, P. D. (in press). Creativity and operant research: Selection and reorganization of responses. In M. A. Runco (Ed.) Handbook of creativity research. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, P. D. (2001). Variability, constraints, and creativity. American Psychologist, 56, 355–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenaar, W. A. (1972). Generation of random sequences by human subjects: A critical survey of literature. Psychological Bulletin, 77, 65–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, R. L. (1964). On producing random responses. Psychological Reports, 14, 931–941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. W. (1980). Reinforcement, behavior constraint, and the overjustification effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 599–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Neuringer, A. (2003). Creativity and Reinforced Variability. In: Lattal, K.A., Chase, P.N. (eds) Behavior Theory and Philosophy. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-4590-0_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-4590-0_17

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4419-3405-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-4590-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics