Recollections of Trauma pp 531-540 | Cite as
Investigating Alternative Accounts of Veridical and Non-Veridical Memories of Trauma
Chapter
Abstract
We summarize the discussions of the two Cognitive Working Groups of the NATO ASI meeting on memory and trauma. The groups found that an understanding of the processes that contribute to veridical and nonveridical memories of trauma will require clarification of the relationship between clinical and cognitive constructs, and a variety of research approaches. The groups concluded that advancement will require greater communication and collaboration among researchers interested in memory of trauma
Keywords
Ptsd Symptom Traumatic Experience False Memory Autobiographical Memory Implicit Memory
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.
Preview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
References
- Anderson, R.C., Pitcher, J.W. (1978). Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 1–12.Google Scholar
- Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A., Bjork, E.I. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 20 (5), 1063–1087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Atkinson, R.C., Herrmann, D.J. Westcourt, K.T. (1974) Search processes in recognition memory. In R.L. Solso (Ed.) Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 101–146 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Atkinson, R. C., Shiffrin, R. M. (1969). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. Spence J. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation ((Vol. 2 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
- Brown, R., Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bjork, R.A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H.L. Roediger., F.I.M. Craik. (Eds). Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving. (pp. 309–330 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ebbinghaus, H. ( 1913 (1885). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Columbia Teacher’s College.Google Scholar
- Eich, J.E. (1980) The cue dependent nature of state dependent retrieval Memory and Cognition, 8, 157–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Erdelyi, M.H., Kleinbard, J. (1978). Has Ebbinghaus decayed with time ? The growth of recall (hypermnesia) over days. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 275–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Freud, S. (1901/1974). Childhood memories and screen memories. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 6, pp. 43–52) London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
- Freud, S. (1915/1957). Repression. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14 pp.141–158). London, Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
- Hilgard, E.R. (1992) Dissociation and theories of hypnosis. In Fromm, E. Nash, M.R. Contemporary Hypnosis Research N.Y., N.Y.: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
- Horowitz, M. J., Reidboard, S. P. (1992). Memory, emotion, and response to trauma. In S. Christianson (Ed.), The Handbook of Emotion and Memory: Research and Theory (pp. 343–357 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Hyman, I. E., Jr., Husband, T. H., Billings, J. F. (1995). False memories of childhood experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 181–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hyman, I. E., Jr., Pentland, J. (1996). Guided imagery and the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- J. W. Schooler and I. E. Hyman, Jr. Janet, P. (1889). L’automatisme continue. Revue génerale des sciences, 4, 167–179Google Scholar
- Jacoby, L.L., Dallas, M. (1981) On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 3, 306–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jacoby, L.L., Kelley, C.M., Dywan, J. Memory Attributions. in H.L. Roediger and F.I.M. Craik (Eds) Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving. (pp. 391–422)Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.Google Scholar
- Loftus, E. F., Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720–725.Google Scholar
- Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of a previous occurrence. Psychological Review, 87, 252–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Neisser, U. (1982). Memory observed: Remembering in natural context. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
- Neisser, U. Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of ‘flashbulb memories“ (pp. 9–31). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Roediger, H.L. (1990). Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory. American Journal of Psychology. 103 (3), pp. 403–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Roediger, H.L., Weldon, M.S., Challis, B.H. (1989) Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In Roediger, H.L., Craik, F.I.M. (Eds) Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving. (pp. 1–41 ). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Schacter. D. (1987). Implicit Memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 13 (3), 510–518.Google Scholar
- Schacter, D., Chiu, C.Y.P., Ohcsner, K.N. (1993). Implicit memory: A selective review. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 159–182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Singer, J.L (1993) Experimental studies of ongoing conscious experience. In Bock, G.R Marsh, J (Eds) Experimental and Theoretical Studies of Consciousness, N.Y., N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
- Ten, L. C., Bloch, D. A., Michel, B. A., Shi, H., Reinhardt, J. A., Metayer, S. (1996). Children’s memories in the wake of Challenger. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 618–625.Google Scholar
- Tromp, S., Koss, M.P., Figueredo, A.J. Tharan, M. (1995). Are rape memories different? A comparison of rape, other unpleasant, and pleasant memories among employed women. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 607–628.Google Scholar
- Tulving, E., Thompson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van der Kolk, B.A., Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: Overview and exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 505–525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van der Kolk, B.A., van der Hart, O. (1991). The intrusive past: The flexibility of memory and the engraving of trauma. American Imago, 48, 425–454.Google Scholar
- Wegner, D. (1994) Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Winograd, E. Neisser, U. (1992), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of ‘flashbulb memories.“ Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Copyright information
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1997