Age Changes in Deceiving and Detecting Deceit

  • Bella M. DePaulo
  • Audrey Jordan

Abstract

Infants’ and children’s initial attempts to decipher the true nature of their interpersonal and physical worlds mark the beginning of a scientific enterprise that will last a lifetime. Such an awesome undertaking requires sophisticated privately-owned equipment (for example, developing cognitive structures and sensory capabilities) and dependable external sources of support (for example, the existence of discernible invariances in the real world). Another tremendously rich source of data consists of information that is conveyed by other people. Much of this information that is conveyed to infants and children is sensible and useful. For example, adults often try to convey to children the names of people and things, the meanings of words, and some notion of the currently acceptable standards of conduct and systems of values. On other occasions, however, adults tell children about verbally fluent bears and pigs; about wolves who dress up like grandmothers; black-clad women who travel by broomstick; and cows that jump over the moon. The child’s world is punctuated with other types of verbal oddities as well. For example, when Junior smashes a priceless antique in his haste to escape to the great outdoors, his father remarks, “That’s just great.” Curiously, his father uttered the exact same words one day when Junior had picked up all of his toys.

Keywords

Facial Expression Nonverbal Behavior Twelfth Grade Mixed Feeling Overt Expression 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramovitch, R. Children’s recognition of situational aspects of facial expressions. Child Development, 1977, 48, 459–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abramovitch, R. and Daly, E. M. Inferring attributes of a situation from the facial expressions of peers. Child Development, 1979, 50, 586–589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Allen, V. L. and Atkinson, M. L. Encoding of nonverbal behavior by high-achieving and low-achieving children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1978, 70, 298–305.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bates, E. Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  5. The one exception to these findings comes from a drink-tasting study involving Korean first graders, seventh graders, and college students (Feldman, 1979 ). In this study, only the seventh grade females leaked. The males used a hamming strategy at every age level, and the strategy became more exaggerated with age. Female first graders also used hamming, while the college students used the natural reproduction strategy. While the details of the findings are in need of further explication, one general conclusion appears to be that the Korean subjects were better able to control their facial expressions than were the American subjects. Age Changes in Deceiving and Detecting Deceit 179Google Scholar
  6. Berko Gleason, J. Code-switching in children’s language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  7. Blanck, P. D., Rosenthal, R., Snodgrass, S. E., DePaulo, B. M., and Zuckerman, M. Sex differences in eavesdropping on nonverbal cues: Developmental changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1981, 41, 391–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bok, S. On lying. Berkshire Review,1980, /5, 7–14.Google Scholar
  9. Braginsky, D. D. Machiavellianism and manipulative interpersonal behavior in children. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,1970, 6,77–99. (a)Google Scholar
  10. Braginsky, D. D. Parent-child correlates of machiavellianism and manipulative behavior. Psychological Reports,1970, 27,927–932.Google Scholar
  11. (b)Christie, R. and Geis, F. L. Studies in machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  12. DePaulo, B. M. Success at detecting deception: Liability or skill? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1981, 364, 245–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. DePaulo, B. M., Jordan, A., Irvine, A., and Laser, P. S. Age changes in the detection of deception. Child Development,1982, 53,in press.Google Scholar
  14. DePaulo, B. M. and Rosenthal, R. Age changes in nonverbal decoding as a function of increasing amounts of information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1978, 26, 280–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. DePaulo, B. M. and Rosenthal, R. Age changes in nonverbal decoding skills: Evidence for increasing differentiation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1979, 25, 145–150.Google Scholar
  16. a) DePaulo, B. M. and Rosenthal, R. Telling lies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,1979, 37,1713–1722. (b)Google Scholar
  17. DePaulo, B. M., Rosenthal, R., Rosenkrantz, J., and Green, C. R. Actual and perceived cues to deception: A closer look at speech. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,in press.Google Scholar
  18. DePaulo, B. M., Zuckerman, M., and Rosenthal, R. Detecting deception: Modality effects. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), The review of personality and social psychology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, 1980.Google Scholar
  19. deVilliers, J. G. and deVilliers, P. A. Language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  20. Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry, 1969, 32, 88–105.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Ekman, P., Roper, G., and Hager, J. C. Deliberate facial movement. Child Development, 1980, 51, 886–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Feffer, M. H. The cognitive implications of role-taking behavior. Journal of Personality, 1959, 27, 152–268.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Feldman, R. S. Nonverbal disclosure of deception in urban Korean adults and children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1979, 10, 73–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Feldman, R. S., Devin-Sheehan, L., and Allen, V. L. Nonverbal cues as indicators of verbal dissembling. American Educational Research Journal, 1978, 15, 217–231.Google Scholar
  25. Feldman, R. S., Jenkins, L., and Popoola, O. Detection of deception in adults and children via facial expressions. Child Development, 1979, 50, 350–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Feldman, R. S. and White, J. B. Detecting deception in children. Journal of Communication,1980, 30, 121–129.Google Scholar
  27. Fisher, J. D., DePaulo, B. M., and Nadler, A. Extending altruism beyond the altruistic act: The mixed effects of aid on the help recipient. In J. R. Rushton and R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior. Hillsdale, NJ; Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
  28. Flavell, J. H. Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1977.Google Scholar
  29. Gardner, H., Kircher, M., Winner, E., and Perkins, D. Children’s metaphoric productions and preferences. Journal of Child Psychology, 1975, 2, 125–141.Google Scholar
  30. Greenberg, M. S. A theory of indebtedness. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, and R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange. New York: Plenum, 1980.Google Scholar
  31. Hartshorne, H. and May, M. A. Studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan, 1928.Google Scholar
  32. Kraut, R. E. Verbal and nonverbal cues in the perception of lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 380–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kraut, R. E. Humans as lie-detectors: Some second thoughts. Journal of Communication, 1980, 30, 209–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kraut, R. E. and Price, J. D. Machiavellianism in parents and their children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1976, 33, 782–786.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Littlepage, G. E. and Pineault, T. Verbal, facial and paralinguistic cues to the detection of truth and lying. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1978, 4, 461–464.Google Scholar
  36. Peevers, B. H. and Secord, P. F. Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 27, 120–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Piaget, J. The moral judgment of the child. New York: The Free Press, 1965.Google Scholar
  38. Rosenthal, R. and DePaulo, B. M. Sex differences in accommodation in nonverbal communication. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication. Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn and Hain, 1979. (a)Google Scholar
  39. Rosenthal, R. and DePaulo, B. M. Sex differences in eavesdropping on nonverbal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 273–285.Google Scholar
  40. (b)Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., and Archer, D. Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
  41. Saarni, C. Children’s understanding of display rules. Developmental Psychology, 1979, 15, 424–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Scheibe, K. E. In defense of lying: On the moral neutrality of misrepresentation. Berkshire Review, 1980, 15, 15–24.Google Scholar
  43. Werner, H. The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1957.Google Scholar
  44. Winner, E., Rosenstiel, A., and Gardner, H. The development of metaphoric understanding. Developmental Psychology, 1976, /2, 287–297.Google Scholar
  45. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., and Rosenthal, R. Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14 ). New York: Academic Press, 1981.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Bella M. DePaulo
  • Audrey Jordan

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations