Mental Load, Mental Effort and Attention

  • G. Mulder
Part of the NATO Conference Series book series (NATOCS, volume 8)

Abstract

Many of the concepts introduced into the research on mental load seem to have developed by analogy with those already available in the field of physical workload.

Keywords

Warning Signal Internal Model Primary Task Secondary Task Mental Effort 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Attneave, F., Applications of information theory to psychology, New York: Holt, 1959.Google Scholar
  2. Bainbridge, L., Problems in the assessment of mental load, Le travail Humain, 1974, 37, 279–302.Google Scholar
  3. Bahrick, H.P., and Shelly, C., Time sharing as an index of automization, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 56, 288–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bahrick, H.P., Noble, M., and Fitts, P.M. Extra-task performance as a measure of learning a primary task, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1954, 48, 298–302.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berdina, M.A., Kolenko, O.L., Kotz, I.M., Kuzetzoy, A.P., Rodinoy, I.M.,Savchenko, A.P. and Thorevsky, V.I. Increase is skeletal muscle performance during emotional stress in man, Circulation Research, 1972, 6, 642–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bobrow, D.G. and Norman, D.A. Some principles of memory schemata, in: D.G. Bobrow and A. Colling, Representation and Understanding, studies in cognitive science, New York: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  7. Broadbent, D.E. Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1977, 29, 181–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brod, J., Neural factors in essential hypertension, in: A. Zanchetti Casa (ed.); Neural and Psychological Mechanisms in Cardiovascular Disease, Milan: Editrice “Il Ponte”, 19 72.Google Scholar
  9. Brown, I.D. The measurement of perceptual load and reserve capacity. Transactions of the Association of Industrial Medical Officers, 1964, 14, 44–49.Google Scholar
  10. Brown, I.D. Subjective and objective comparisons of successful and unsuccessful trainee drivers, Ergonomics, 1966, 8, 467–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brown, I.D. and Poulton, E.C. Measuring the spare “mental” capacity of cardrivers by a subsidiary task, Ergonomics, 1961, 4, 35–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cannon, W.B. Bodily changes in pain-hunger fear and rage, Boston: Branford, 1929.Google Scholar
  13. Caille, E.J. and Kessler, D. A taxonomic review of the place of the operator within a man-machine system, in: Singleton: The Measurement of Human Resources, London, Taylor and Francis, 1976.Google Scholar
  14. Coles, M.G.H. and Duncan-Johnson, C.C. Cardiac Activity and Information Processing: The effects of stimulus significance and detection and response requirements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975, 1, 418–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Curry, R.E. and Gai, E.G. Detection of random process failures by human monitors, in: T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring Behavior and Supervisory Control, New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  16. Deecke, L., Grozinger, B. and Kornhuber, H.H., Voluntary Finger Movement in Man: Cerebral Potentials and Theory, Biol. Cybernetics, 1976, 23, 99–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Donders, F.C. On the speed of mental processes (1869). Trans. W.G. Koster in: W.G. Koster (ed.), Attention and Performance II, Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Co., 1969.Google Scholar
  18. Duffy, E. Activation, in: N.S. Greenfield and R.A. Sternbach, Handbook of Psychophysiology, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. 1973.Google Scholar
  19. Frankenhauser, M. Sympathetic-Adrenomedullary Activity, Behavior, and the Psychosocial Environment, in: P.H. Venables and M.J. Christie, Research in Psychophysiology, London: John Wiley and Sons, 1975.Google Scholar
  20. Gaillard, A.W.K. Effects on warning-signal modality on the CNV, Biological Psychology, 1976, 4, 139–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gaillard, A.W.K. The terminal CNV: Preparation versus expectancy, Psychophysiology, 1977, 14.Google Scholar
  22. Gaillard, A.W.K. Slow brain potentials preceding discrimination performance, Biological Psychology, submitted, 1978.Google Scholar
  23. Gruzelier, J.H. The cardiac responses of schizophrenics to orienting, signal and non-signal tones. Biological Psychology, 1975, 3, 143–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hahn, W.W. The hypothesis of Lacey: A critical appraisal, Psychological Bulletin, 1973, 79, 59–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hamilton, P. Activation States and Components of Human Performance, in: S. Dornic, Attention and Performance VI, New York, Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  26. Hamilton, V. Socialisation anxiety and information processing: a capacity model of anxiety-induced performance deficits, in: I.G. Sarason and C.D. Spielberger (eds.), Stress and Anxiety, II, Washington D.C., Hemisphere Publishing, 1975.Google Scholar
  27. Hover, K.I. A developmental study of three components of attention, Developmental Psychology, 1974, 10, 330–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Johannsen, G., Pfendler, C. and Stein, W. Human performance and workload in simulated landing-approaches with autopilot-failures, in T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring behavior and supervisory control, New York, Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  29. Kahneman, D. Attention and Effort, New Jersey: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1973.Google Scholar
  30. Keele, S.W. Attention and Human Performance, Pacific Palisades, California: Goodyear Publishing Company Inc., 1973.Google Scholar
  31. Klein, R.M. Attention and Movement, in: G.E. Stelmach, Motor Control, New York, Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  32. Knowles, W.B. Operator loading tasks, Human Factors, 1963, 5, 151–161.Google Scholar
  33. Lacey, J.I. Somatic response patterning and stress: some revisions of activation theory, in: M.A. Appley and R. Trumbull (eds.), Psychological stress: Issues in research, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.Google Scholar
  34. Lacey, J.I., Lacey, B.C. Some autonomic central nervous system interrelationships, in: P. Black (ed.), Physiological Correlates of Emotion, New York: Academic Press, 1970, 205–227.Google Scholar
  35. Lindsley, B.D. The role of nonspecific reticulothalama cortical systems in emotion, in P. Black: Physiological Correlates of Emotion, New York: Academic Press, 1970.Google Scholar
  36. Loveless, N.E. and Sanford, A.J. Slow potential correlates of preparatory set, Biological Psychology, 1974, 1, 303–314.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mason, J.W. Organization of Psychodendrocrine Mechanisms: A Review and Reconsideration of Research, in: Singleton: The Measurement of human resources, London, Taylor and Francis, 1976.Google Scholar
  38. Mandler, G. Consciousness: respectable, useful, and probably necessary, in: R.L. Solso, Information processing and cognition, Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.Google Scholar
  39. McGuinness, D. Cardiovascular responses during habituation and mental activity in anxious men and women. Biological Psychology, 1973, 1, 115–123.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Michon, J.A. The problem of perceptual motor load, in: Studies in Perception. Dedicated to M.A. Bouman. Soesterberg: Institute for Perception, RVO-TNO, 1966.Google Scholar
  41. Michon, J.A. Tapping regularity as a measure of perceptual motor load. Ergonomics, 1966, 9, 401–412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Moray, N. Attention, control and sampling behaviour. In T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring behavior and supervisory control, New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  43. Moray, N. Attention: selective processes in vision and hearing, London: Hutchinson Education Ltd, 1969.Google Scholar
  44. Mulder, G., Michon, J.A., Moraal, J. Motor skills, in: J.A. Michon, E.G.J. Eijkman and L.F.W. de Klerk (eds.), Handbook of Psychonomics, Deventer: Van Loghum and Slaterus, 1978.Google Scholar
  45. Naatanen, R., The inverted - U relationship between activation and performance: a critical review, in: S. Kornblum, Attention and Performance, IV, New York, Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  46. Neisser, U., Cognitive Psychology, New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts, 1967.Google Scholar
  47. Neisser, U. Cognition and Reality, San Francisco: Freeman, 1976.Google Scholar
  48. Norman, D.A. and Bobrow, D.G. On data-limited and resource- limited processes, Cognitive Psychology, 1975, 7, 44–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Obrist, P.A., Webb, R.A., Sutterer, J.R. and Howard, J.L. Cardiac deceleration and reaction time: An evaluation of two hypotheses, Psychophysiology, 1970, 6, 695–706.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Pew, R.W. Levels of analysis in motor control, Brain Research, 1974, 71, 393–400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pew, R.W. Human perceptual-motor performance, in: B.H. Kantowitz: Human Information Processing: Tutorials in Performance and Cognition, New York: John Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  52. Posner, M.I. Psychobiology of attention, in: M.S. Gazzaniga and C. Blakemore (eds.), Handbook of Psychobiology, New York, London: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  53. Posner, M.I., Nissen, M.J. and Klein, R.M. Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 1976, 83, 157–171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Pribram, K.H., and McGuinness, D. Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention, Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 116–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Prechtl, H.F.R. The Behavioral State of the Newborn Infant (A Review), Brain Research, 1974, 76, 185–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rasmussen, J. Outlines of a hybrid model of the process plant operator in: T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring behavior and supervisory control, New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  57. Rolfe, J.M. The secondary task as a measure of mental load, in: W.T. Singleton, J.G. Fox and D.W. Whitfield, Measure- ment of Man at Work, London: Taylor and Francis Ltd., 1971.Google Scholar
  58. Rolfe, J.M. The measurement of human response in man-vehicle control situations, in: T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring Behavior and Supervisory Control, New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  59. Sanders, A.F. Structural and functional aspects of the reaction process, in S. Dornic (ed.), Attention and Performance VI, New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  60. Schmidt, R.A. A Schema Theory of Discrete Motor Skill Learning, Psychological Review, 1975, 82, 225–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Schmidt, R.A. The Schema as a Solution to Some Persistent Problems in Motor Learning Theory, in: G.E. Stelmach, Motor Control, New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  62. Selye, H. Stress, Montreal: ACTA, 1950.Google Scholar
  63. Shiffrin, R.M. Capacity limitations in information processing, attention and memory, in: W.K. Estes (ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 4 ), Hillsdale N.J.: Erlbaum, 1976.Google Scholar
  64. Shiffrin, R.M. and Schneider, W. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing, Psychological Review, 1977, 84, 127–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Smallwood, R.D. Internal models and the human instrument monitor, IEEE Transactions on Human Factors in Electronics, 1967, HFE 8.Google Scholar
  66. Stelmach, G.E. Motor Control. New York: Academic Press, 1976.Google Scholar
  67. Veldhuyzen, W. and Stassen, H.G. The internal modei: what does it mean in human control, in: T.B. Sheridan and G. Johannsen, Monitoring Behavior and Supervisory Control, New York: Plenum Press, 1976.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1979

Authors and Affiliations

  • G. Mulder
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of GroningenNetherlands

Personalised recommendations