Advertisement

Sneezing Behavior in the Squirrel Monkey and Its Biological Significance

  • Gary G. Schwartz
  • Leonard A. Rosenblum

Abstract

When Ernst Mayr (1961) declared that there are “two biologies,” a “How does it work?” and a “Why is it so?” biology, he stressed the division between two natural sciences: functional and evolutionary biology. Functional biologists study “proximate” causes—mostly physiological processes—and are not particularly concerned with the history or meaning of the genetic programs of the organisms that possess them. Evolutionary biologists, on the other hand, study “ultimate” causes—biological diversity and its meaning—and are much vexed by such questions as, “Why are there lions and tigers and things?” (Waddington, 1957). A familiar tale by the Brothers Grimm may illustrate the viewpoints of the two biologies. Little Red Riding Hood’s observation, “Grandma, What big teeth you have!,” exemplifies the view of a functional biologist; the Wolf’s reply, “The better to eat you with,” the point of an evolutionary biologist.

Keywords

Cavernous Sinus Squirrel Monkey World Monkey Nasal Secretion Functional Biologist 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andersen, I. B., Lindquist, G. R., and Proctor, D. F., 1972, Human nasal mucosal function under four controlled humidities, Am. Rev. Respir. Dis. 106: 438–449.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews, R. V., 1983, Hyperthermic reactions of voles to novel and repetitive psychogenic stimuli, Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 75A: 589–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker, M. A., 1980, Anatomical and physiological adaptations of panting mammals to heat and exercise, in: Environmental Physiology: Aging, Heat and Altitude ( S. Horvath and Y. Yousef, eds.), Elsevier/North-Holland, New York, pp. 121–146.Google Scholar
  4. Baker, M. A., 1982, Brain cooling in endotherms in heat and exercise, Ann. Rev. Physiol. 44: 8596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baker, M. A., Cronin, M. J., and Mountjoy, D. G., 1976, Variability of skin temperature in the waking monkey, Am. J. Physiol. 230: 449–455.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bantin, G. C., 1966, Establishment of a squirrel monkey colony, J. Inst. Anim. Tech. 17:66–73. Barnett, S. A., 1981, Modern Ethology: The Science of Animal Behavior, Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
  7. Bartholomew, G. A., 1977, Body temperature and energy metabolism, in: Animal Physiology: Principles and Adaptations ( M. S. Gordon, ed.), Macmillan, New York, pp. 364–449.Google Scholar
  8. Brockie, R., 1976, Self-anointing by wild hedgehogs, Erinaceus europaeus in New Zealand, Anim. Behay. 24: 68–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cabanac, M., and Caputa, M., 1979, Natural selective cooling of the human brain: Evidence of its occurrence and magnitude, J. Physiol. 286: 255–264.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Caputa, M., Kadziela, W., and Narebski, J., 1976, Significance of cranial circulation for the brain homeothermia in rabbits. II. The role of the cranial venous lakes in the defense against hyperthermia, Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 36: 625–638.Google Scholar
  11. Curio, E., 1973, Towards a methodology of teleonomy, Experientia 29: 1045–1058.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Darwin, C., 1868, Hedgehogs, in: Hardwicke’s Science Gossip: An Illustrated Medium of Inter-change and Gossip for Students and Lovers of Nature p. 280. [Reprinted in P. H. Barett, ed.,1977, The Collected Papers of Charles Darwin, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, p. 137.]Google Scholar
  13. Deinhart, J. B., Devine, J., Passovoy, M., Pohlman, R., and Deinhart, F., 1967, Marmosets as laboratory animals, Lab. Anim. Care 17: 11–29.Google Scholar
  14. Eisenberg, J. F 1981, The Mammalian Radiations, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  15. Eisenberg, J. F., and Kleiman, D. G., 1972, Olfactory communication in mammals, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 3: 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gardner, M., 1981, The annotated Snark, in: Lewis Carroll’s The Hunting of the Snark ( J. Tanis and J. Dooley, eds.), William Kaufman, Los Altos, California, pp. 13–80.Google Scholar
  17. Ghiselin, M. T., 1978, The economy of the body, Am. Economic Rev. 68: 233–237.Google Scholar
  18. Gould, S. J., 1975, Allometry in primates, with an emphasis on scaling and the evolution of the brain, in: Contributions to Primatology, Volume 5, pp. 244–292.Google Scholar
  19. Hamilton, W. F., Woodbury, R. A., and Harper, H. T., Jr., 1944, Arterial, cerebrospinal and venous pressures in man during cough and strain, Am. J. Physiol. 141: 42–50.Google Scholar
  20. Hennessy, M. B., Mendoza, S. P., Coe, C. L., Lowe, E. L., and Levine, S., 1980, Androgen-related behavior in the squirrel monkey: An issue that is nothing to sneeze at, Behay. Neural Biol. 30: 103–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hull, D., 1974, Philosophy of Biological Science, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  22. Huxley, T. H., 1929, On the method of Zadig, in: Essays of Thomas H. Huxley ( F. Barry, ed.), Macmillan, New York, pp. 160–180.Google Scholar
  23. Jolies, J., Rompa-Barendregt, J., and Gispen, W. H., 1979, Novelty and grooming behavior in the rat, Behay. Neural Biol. 25: 563–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Levy, B. M., and Artecona, J., 1964, The marmoset as an experimental animal in biological research: Care and maintenance, Lab. Anim. Care 14: 20–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Mahoney, S. A., 1980, Cost of locomotion and heat balance during rest and running from 0 to 55°C in a patas monkey, J. Appl. Physiol. Respir. Environ. Exercise Physiol. 49: 789–800.Google Scholar
  26. Maynard Smith, J., 1982, Overview—Unsolved evolutionary problems, in: Genome Evolution ( G.A. Dover and R. B. Flavell, eds. Academic Press, New York, pp. 375–382.Google Scholar
  27. Mayr, E., 1961, Cause and effect in biology, Science 134: 1501–1506. [Reprinted in E. Mayr, 1976, Evolution and the Diversity of Life, Belknap Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 359–371.]Google Scholar
  28. McLanahan, E. B., and Green, K. M., 1977, The vocal repertoire and analysis of the contexts of vocalizations in Leontopithecus rosalia, in: The Biology and Conservation of the Callitrichidae(D. G. Kleiman, ed.), Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D. C., pp. 251–269.Google Scholar
  29. Mount, L. E., 1979, Adaptation to Thermal Environment, Edward Arnold, London.Google Scholar
  30. Moynihan, M., 1955, Types of hostile display, Auk 72: 247–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Moynihan, M., 1964, Some behavior patterns of Platyrrhine monkeys I. The night monkey (Aotus trivirgatus), Smithson. Misc. Coll. 146: 1–84.Google Scholar
  32. Moynihan, M., 1966, Communication in the titi monkey, Callicebus, J. Zool. (Lond.) 150: 77–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Moynihan, M., 1970, Some behavior patterns of Platyrrhine monkeys. II. Saguinus geoffroyi and some other tamarins, Smithson. Contrib. Zool. 28: 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Moynihan, M., 1976, The New World Monkeys, Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  35. Robinson, J. G., 1980, Correlates of urine washing in the wedge-capped capuchin Cebus nigrivit-tatus, in: Vertebrate Ecology in the Northern Neotropics, (J. F. Eisenberg, ed.), Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D. C., pp. 137–143.Google Scholar
  36. Schmidt, U., and Seitz, E., 1967, Washen mit Ham zum Zweck der Thermoregulation bei Totenkopfaffen (Saimiri sciureus), Anthropol. Anz. 30: 162–165.Google Scholar
  37. Schwartz, G. G., 1981, Thermally-induced sneezing in the squirrel monkey: An hypothesis for the origins of platyrrhine sneezing, Am. J. Primatol. 3: 325–326 (Abstract).Google Scholar
  38. Schwartz, G. G., 1985, Thermal Influences on the Behavior of Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus): Does Saimiri Sneeze to Thermoregulate?, Ph.D. Dissertation, Downstate Medical Center of the State University of New York (unpublished).Google Scholar
  39. Schwartz, G. G., and Rosenblum, L. A., 1980, Novelty, arousal, and nasal marking in the squirrel monkey, Behay. Neural Biol. 28: 116–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Schwartz, G. G., and Rosenblum, L. A., 1983, Allometric influences on primate mothers and infants, in: Symbiosis in Parent—Offspring Interactions ( L. A. Rosenblum and H. Moltz, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 215–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sharpey-Schafer, E. P., 1953, Effects of coughing on intrathoracic pressure, arterial pressure and peripheral blood flow, J. Physiol. 122: 351–357.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Thiessen, D. D., 1977, Thermoenergetics and the evolution of pheromone communication, in: Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, Volume III ( J. M. Sprague and A. N. Epstein, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 91–191.Google Scholar
  43. Thiessen, D. D., 1983, The thermoenergetics of communication and social interaction among Mongolian gerbils, in: Symbiosis in Parent—Offspring Interactions ( L. A. Rosenblum and H. Moltz, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 113–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Thorington, R. W., Jr., 1968, Observations of squirrel monkeys in a Colombian forest, in: The Squirrel Monkey, ( L. A. Rosenblum and R. W. Cooper, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 69–85.Google Scholar
  45. Waddington, C. H., 1957, The Strategy of the Genes, Allen / Unwin, London.Google Scholar
  46. Williams, G. C., 1966, Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought,Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gary G. Schwartz
    • 1
  • Leonard A. Rosenblum
    • 1
  1. 1.Primate Behavior Laboratory and Department of PsychiatryDownstate Medical CenterBrooklynUSA

Personalised recommendations