Attention to Action

Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior
  • Donald A. Norman
  • Tim Shallice

Abstract

Much effort has been made to understand the role of attention in perception; much less effort has been placed on the role attention plays in the control of action. Our goal in this chapter is to account for the role of attention in action, both when performance is automatic and when it is under deliberate conscious control. We propose a theoretical framework structured around the notion of a set of active schemas, organized according to the particular action sequences of which they are a part, awaiting the appropriate set of conditions so that they can become selected to control action. The analysis is therefore centered around actions, primarily external actions, but the same principles apply to internal actions—actions that involve only the cognitive processing mechanisms. One major emphasis in the study of attentional processes is the distinction between controlled and automatic processing of perceptual inputs (e.g., Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). Our work here can be seen as complementary to the distinction between controlled and automatic processes: we examine action rather than perception; we emphasize the situations in which deliberate, conscious control of activity is desired rather than those that are automatic.

Keywords

Action Sequence Attentional Resource Dual Task Psychological Refractory Period Human Information Processing 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ach, N. (1905). liber die Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Gottingen: Vardenhoek.Google Scholar
  2. Allport, D. A. (1980). Attention and performance. In G. L. Claxton (Ed.), New directions in cognitive psychology. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  3. Allport, D. A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bellman, K. (1979). The conflict behavior of the lizard, Sceloporus Occidentalis, and its implication for the organization of motor behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
  5. Boden, M. (1977). Artificial intelligence and natural man. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  6. Denny-Brown, D., & Yanagisawa, N. (1976). The role of the basal ganglia in the initiation of movement. In M. D. YAHR (Ed.), The basal ganglia. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  7. Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 272–300.Google Scholar
  8. Ells, J. G. (1973). Analysis of temporal and attentional aspects of movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 99, 10–21.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fuster, J. M. (1980). The prefrontal cortex: New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  10. Greenwald, A. G., & Shulman, A. G. (1973). On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, 70–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Helson, H., & Steger, J. A. (1962). On the inhibitory effect of a second stimulus following the primary stimulus to react. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 201–205.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  14. Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. M. (1983). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. PARASURAMAN, R. DAVIES, & J. BEATTY (Eds.), Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Keele, S. W. (1973). Attention and human performance. Pacific Palisades, CA: Goodyear.Google Scholar
  16. Lhermitte, F., Derouesne, J., & Signoret, J-L. (1972). Analyse neuropsychologique du syndrome frontale. Revue Neuropsychologique, 127, 415–440.Google Scholar
  17. Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
  18. Lyon, M., & Robbins, T. (1975). The action of central nervous system drugs: A general theory concerning amphetamine effects. In W. B. ESSMANN & L. VALZELLI (Eds.), Current developments in psychopharmacology, Vol. 2. New York: Spectrum.Google Scholar
  19. Marsden, C. D. (1982). The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia. Neurology, 32, 514–539.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McDermott, J., & Forgy, C. (1978). Production system conflict resolution strategies. In D. A. WATERMAN & F. HAYES-ROTH (Eds.), Pattern-directed inference systems. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  22. McFie, J. (1960). Psychological testing in clinical neurology. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 131, 383–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. McLean, J. P., & Shulman, G. L. (1978). On the construction and maintenance of expectancies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 441–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. McLeod, P. D. (1977). A dual task response modality effect: Support for multiprocessor models of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 651–658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McLeod, P. D. (1980). What can probe RT tell us about the attentional demands of movement? In G. E. STELMACH & J. REQUIN (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  26. Milner, B. (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J. M. WARREN & K. AKERT (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  27. Milner, B. (1982). Some cognitive effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B., 298, 211–226.Google Scholar
  28. Nelson, H. (1976). A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex, 12, 313–324.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pew, R. W. (1974). Human perceptual motor performance. In B. H. KANTOWITZ (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Pillsbury, W. B. (1908). Attention. London: Swan Sonnenschein.Google Scholar
  32. Posner, M. I., & Keele; S. W. (1969). Attention demands of movement. In Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Applied Psychology. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger. POSNER, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned. In G. UNDERWOOD & R. STEVENS (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  34. Reason, J. T. (1983). Lapses of attention. In R. PARASURAMAN, R. DAVIES, & J. BEATTY (Eds.), Varieties of Attention. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  35. Reason, J. T., & Mycielska, K. (1982). Absentminded? The psychology of mental lapses and everyday errors. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  36. Robbins, T. W., & Sahakian, B. (1983). Behavioral effects of psychomotor drugs: Clinical and neuropsychological implications. In I. CREESE (Ed.) Stimulants: Neurochemical, behavioral and clinical perspectives. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  37. Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1982). Simulating a skilled typist: A study of skilled cognitive-motor performance. Cognitive Science, 6, 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rylander, G. (1939). Personality changes after operations on the frontal lobes. Acta Psychiatrica Neurologica Scandinavica, Supplement no. 30.Google Scholar
  39. Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 298, 199–209.Google Scholar
  41. Shallice, T., McLeod, P. D., & Lewis, K. (1985). Isolating cognitive modules with the dual task paradigm: Are speech perception and production separate processes? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (in press).Google Scholar
  42. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 205230.Google Scholar
  44. Treisman,. A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Walsh, K. W. (1978). Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Donald A. Norman
    • 1
  • Tim Shallice
    • 2
  1. 1.Institute for Cognitive ScienceUniversity of California, San DiegoLa JollaUSA
  2. 2.Applied Psychology UnitMedical Research CouncilCambridgeEngland

Personalised recommendations