Skip to main content

Neurolinguistic Discourse

  • Chapter
  • 193 Accesses

Part of the book series: Applied Psycholinguistics and Communication Disorders ((APCD))

Abstract

Thirty years after Marie's statement, Luria comes back to the same theme in his Travrnaticheskaya Afaziya:1

As a rule brain lesions inflicted by firearms... provide one the opportunity to observe the effects of quite limited lesions. ([1643], p. 26)

Injuries of the skull by weapons of war, causing superficial wounds of the cerebral cortex underneath, allow us to appreciate — often with remarkable closeness — the localization of the various brain centres.

Marie [254] (1917)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Published in the USSR in 1947.

    Google Scholar 

  2. See p. 177, n. 83.

    Google Scholar 

  3. See p. 152, n. 84.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Auguste-Henri Forel (1848-1933). His contribution can be summed up in three points: (1) the nervous network does not exist as such, and each nerve cell is in contact but not in continuity with its neighbours. This is what is called Forel’s contact theory.(2) Nerve fibres originate from the cell. (3) As a result, fibre degenerates if the cell is damaged, and pace Waller, the converse may also occur.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1852-1934).

    Google Scholar 

  6. H. Wilhelm Gottfried von Waldeyer (1836-1921). He in fact gave a new meaning to the Greek term long used for “nerve”.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Charles Scott Sherrington (1857-1952). The term “synapse” was also used in 1897 by M. Forster.

    Google Scholar 

  8. E. H. Weber (1795-1878) and E.F.W. Weber (1806-1871). Their experiment is described in the Handwörterbuch der Physiologie mit Rücksicht auf physiologische Pathologie (Braunschweig, 1846, Vol. III, pp. 1-222:‘Muskelbewegung’). They showed that Stimulation of the pneumogastric nerve halted the heartbeat; in other words, the activity of a nerve caused the interruption of the activity of the organ innervated by it.

    Google Scholar 

  9. To express the phenomenon demonstrated by the Brothers Weber, Brown-Sequard pressed into service the term “inhibition” in allusion to a legal sense: “put in opposition”.

    Google Scholar 

  10. See p.152, n. 105.

    Google Scholar 

  11. J.C. Eccles (1903-): [256].

    Google Scholar 

  12. See [la,b] and [188].

    Google Scholar 

  13. [256], p. 37.

    Google Scholar 

  14. “…The neuron appears to us as a cell endowed with various functions; but all these functions are organised to ensure the transmission of information from one point of the organism to another…” ([256], p. 37).

    Google Scholar 

  15. Hans Berger (1873-1941). See [65].

    Google Scholar 

  16. Pierre Chauchard [70], pp. 4 and 64.

    Google Scholar 

  17. [250a], pp. 480-492.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Ivan Petrovitch Pavlov (1849-1936).

    Google Scholar 

  19. [246d], p. 20.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Wilder Graves Penfield (1891-1976).

    Google Scholar 

  21. “An injection of sodium amytal made into the inner carotid artery causes a contralateral hemiplegia” ([307], pp. 89-90). See [51a], pp. 52 and 164.

    Google Scholar 

  22. “Cortical exaeresis… did not abolish the ability to speak in the patient, although aphasia frequently appeared as a postoperative reaction, to disappear again when convalescence was complete” ([307], p. 110).

    Google Scholar 

  23. See [420b].

    Google Scholar 

  24. Penfield [305a].

    Google Scholar 

  25. In [306], p. 215.

    Google Scholar 

  26. “The optic layer or thalamus is an ovoid nucleus of grey matter whose internal and superior surfaces protrude into visibility from the third ventricle and the lateral ventricle, and whose external and inferior surfaces are attached to neighbouring regions. The posterior extremity presents a voluminous inflation: the pulvinar, reaching out over two eminences, the external and internal geniculate bodies, where the conjunctival arms end.” ([98], p. 102). This complex set of different structures is referred to as the limbic system. See [19], p. 10.

    Google Scholar 

  27. This is a short but crucial text among Penfield’s writings. See [305b].

    Google Scholar 

  28. We are here following the text of [307].

    Google Scholar 

  29. The word “abiding” is borrowed from the oft-quoted description by Sherrington (Adrian, 1947, p. 17) ([307], p. 55, n. 1).

    Google Scholar 

  30. [307], p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  31. [307], p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  32. [307], p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  33. The Harvey Lectures, New York Academy of Medicine, 1938. Title: “The Cerebral Cortex and Consciousness”.

    Google Scholar 

  34. [307], pp. 19f.

    Google Scholar 

  35. See in particular Jasper (1960) ([200b], pp. 1552-1593). We might add that research up till 1975 in this area is well summed up in a special issue of Brain and Language 2, 1 (1975).

    Google Scholar 

  36. See p. 49.

    Google Scholar 

  37. A. R. Luria ([246k], p. 55). On the concept of regulative centre, see Luria [246fJ, part 3, “Synthetic mental activities and their cerebral organisation”, pp. 245ff.

    Google Scholar 

  38. See [246k], pp. 55f.

    Google Scholar 

  39. [246a], p. 21.

    Google Scholar 

  40. [246a], p. 21.

    Google Scholar 

  41. On this aspect of the problem, see Luria [246c]. See also Luria and Yudovich [247].

    Google Scholar 

  42. [246a], p. 21.

    Google Scholar 

  43. “On the basis of all this there is no reason to consider the semantic aspect of speech to result from the functioning of some cortical‘center’ which acts as a‘depot’ for those images which language dominates. We have every reason to believe that the speech activity associated with abstraction and generalisation is a product of highly complex systems of temporary connections which arise in the process of communication with other people” ([246a], p. 22).

    Google Scholar 

  44. See p. 198, n. 50.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Four volumes have appeared to date: I and II (1976), III (1977), IV (1979). Space does not allow us to summarize these volumes, but they are rich in content [420A].

    Google Scholar 

  46. See n. 26.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Yalow, Guillemin and Schally, who devised this method, received the Nobel Prize for their work in 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  48. See p. 186.

    Google Scholar 

  49. [253e], p. 135. See [125].

    Google Scholar 

  50. “All that is said here is aimed at a psychic centre of speech, but as regards the mechanism of articulation, we must state that it may be impaired by a lesion sited in either hemisphere at the level of the‘quadrilateral’ region” ([253e], p. 135).

    Google Scholar 

  51. [253e], p. 136.

    Google Scholar 

  52. A zone that Pierre Marie situated at the level of the gyrus, the gyrus angularis and the first temporals.

    Google Scholar 

  53. [253e], p. 138.

    Google Scholar 

  54. [358b], p. 33.

    Google Scholar 

  55. He says, a few lines previously: “…One might say that it is not spoken language that is natural to man, but the faculty of constituting a language, i.e. a system of distinctive signs corresponding to distinct ideas…” ([358b], p. 26).

    Google Scholar 

  56. It was published in a new edition in 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  57. He adds the following details to this claim in a footnote: “Leyton and Sherrington (1917) observed that they could not produce vocalization in anthropoids by faradic stimulation” ([307], p. 199).

    Google Scholar 

  58. See [72a,b]; the first version of [72d] goes back to early 1967. Chomsky returns to this question in [72ej.

    Google Scholar 

  59. “…In studying the behavior of a complex organism, it will be necessary to isolate such essentially independent underlying systems as the system of linguistic competence…” ([72c], p. 399).

    Google Scholar 

  60. “…A person with command of a language… has developed what we will refer to as linguistic competence” ([72c], p. 397).

    Google Scholar 

  61. [72c], pp. 397f.

    Google Scholar 

  62. [72c], p. 398.

    Google Scholar 

  63. “The existence of innate mental structure is, obviously, not a matter of controversy. What we may question is just what it is and to what extent it is specific to language” ([72c], p. 401).

    Google Scholar 

  64. [72c], p. 402.

    Google Scholar 

  65. For example, Bishop Wilkins ([72c], p. 402).

    Google Scholar 

  66. [72c], p. 407.

    Google Scholar 

  67. [72c], p. 407.

    Google Scholar 

  68. [196b], p. 105.

    Google Scholar 

  69. [196b], p. 105.

    Google Scholar 

  70. [196b], p. 106.

    Google Scholar 

  71. “We may expect to find genetic preconditions for the development of language and mechanisms that regulate language ontogeny, just as there are these genetic and regulatory mechanisms in the development of the structural components of the developing organism” ([196b], p. 107).

    Google Scholar 

  72. See [51a], “Phylogénie et ontogénie”, pp. 138ff., and [196b], pp. 107ff.

    Google Scholar 

  73. “As the cerebral cortex matures postnatally, certain linguistic functions become increasingly restricted to the left cerebral hemisphere” ([196b], p. 111).

    Google Scholar 

  74. [311], p. 67.

    Google Scholar 

  75. “I would say, therefore, that a rational approach would consist of supposing that, in the domain where we have certain non-trivial results concerning language structure, the organizational principles determining the specific structures of language are simply part of the initial state of the organism” ([311], p. 257).

    Google Scholar 

  76. See [246i,e]

    Google Scholar 

  77. Yvan Lebrun [224], p. 1).

    Google Scholar 

  78. Issue 5 of the journal Langages, Paris, March 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  79. “Lesion of the peripheral organs of the bulb or the cerebellum” ([106b], p. 3).

    Google Scholar 

  80. “Lesions localized in the left hemisphere of right-handers and varying with this localization” ([106b], p. 3f.).

    Google Scholar 

  81. “General modifications of behavior of the subject in relation to the world” ([106b], p. 4).

    Google Scholar 

  82. [106b], p. 10.

    Google Scholar 

  83. F. Grewel [165]: “To limit neurolinguistics to aphasiology is inaccurate.”

    Google Scholar 

  84. [246e].

    Google Scholar 

  85. See [420a].

    Google Scholar 

  86. [193d].

    Google Scholar 

  87. [208bj.

    Google Scholar 

  88. “Mere disconnection of right from left hemisphere suffices to reveal at once that the right hemisphere can decode speech to a fairly complex level…”

    Google Scholar 

  89. “It appears that aphasic speech does not necessarily, as has been generally assumed, represent the limited functioning of a damaged domi-nant language area. Rather it may result from the unsophisticated compensatory efforts of the intact minor hemisphere” ([208b], p. 110).

    Google Scholar 

  90. [208a].

    Google Scholar 

  91. We may also mention the Applied Psycholinguistics Series edited for Plenum by R. W. Rieber.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Brain and Language, Neuropsychologia.

    Google Scholar 

  93. This can be demonstrated by a look at the tables of contents of some publications.

    Google Scholar 

  94. See [226a].

    Google Scholar 

  95. “The first step in linguistic analysis of any string of aphasic language is to achieve reliable identification of the transformed unit…” ([226e], p. 193).

    Google Scholar 

  96. See [246k], p. 95.

    Google Scholar 

  97. [255].

    Google Scholar 

  98. [65].

    Google Scholar 

  99. [226a], pp. 212ff.

    Google Scholar 

  100. [226a], p. 219.

    Google Scholar 

  101. “The mind remains a mystery” ([305c], p. 83).

    Google Scholar 

  102. The Neurosciences: Paths of Discovery, Frederick J. Worden, Judith P. Swazey and George Adelman, editors, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass. & London, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  103. [246j], pp. 335-361.

    Google Scholar 

  104. See D. H. Hubel & T. N. Wiesel (1962): “Receptive fields: binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex”: J. Physiol. 160: 106-154.

    Google Scholar 

  105. [9].

    Google Scholar 

  106. [415], p. 341.

    Google Scholar 

  107. [233].

    Google Scholar 

  108. [144].

    Google Scholar 

  109. It may be useful to point out that the word “system”, in Russian sys-tema, is to be taken in its strong sense. “It indicates that there is a multitude of complex interconnexions between the elements (signs and verbal relations) which make of it a very complicated network of liaisons and reciprocal actions; it is indeed a structure” ([231], p. 138).

    Google Scholar 

  110. See above, n. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  111. See p. 178, n. 114.

    Google Scholar 

  112. [148].

    Google Scholar 

  113. [246j], p. 342.

    Google Scholar 

  114. [246j], p. 346.

    Google Scholar 

  115. See n. 102.

    Google Scholar 

  116. See [378]. He was particularly interested in the consequences of hem-ispherectomies and déconnexions of the two hemispheres (the “split brain” phenomenon).

    Google Scholar 

  117. See [305d]. This text was actually incorporated into his book The Mystery of the Mind; see n. 26 (pp. 437-454 of the book).

    Google Scholar 

  118. See [246j], p. 353; [379] and [59], of which we will quote the conclusion: “Finally, it might be said that the proposed model, however briefly depicted, does represent a radical departure from the classical approach. According to the latter, local function was interpreted either as a result of disruption of a mechanism,‘representations’,’ strategies’, etc., or as a pathway-mediated influence upon some other area. The‘centers’ of traditional neuropsychology are rather to be considered as levels by means of which cognition is carried one stage further. Similarly pathways do not serve to associate ideas, perceptions to movements, written words to sounds, etc., but rather link up temporally transformations occurring at different points in the microgenetic sequence. Cognition is a lawful unfolding of evolutionary forms, not a pastiche of more or less fragmentary elements.”

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1991 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bouton, C.P. (1991). Neurolinguistic Discourse. In: Neurolinguistics Historical and Theoretical Perspectives. Applied Psycholinguistics and Communication Disorders. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-9570-0_12

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-9570-0_12

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-9572-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-9570-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics