Skip to main content
Book cover

Ontogeny pp 1–100Cite as

Preformation and Epigenesis in the Origins of the Nervous System and Behavior: Issues, Concepts, and their History

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Ethology ((PEIE,volume 5))

Abstract

One of the most enduring and fundamental debates in the history of biology was over the problem of how in each generation a complex, functioning organism could arise from the vastly more simple fertilized egg derived from the parents. This ancient inquiry into the process of individual development marked the beginning of an interest in ontogenetic issues. Although philosophers and naturalists as far back as Aristotle were concerned with this question, it was not until the 17th and 18th centuries that the problem of development became a matter of sustained and serious interest.

Initially, the two major explanations of the process of development, preformation and epigenesis, appeared to be so different as to be virtually irreconcilable. Preformation theory argued that the fertilized egg, although apparently formless and unorganized, contained a full set of adult organs and characters that were too small to be seen. Thus, what appeared to be the de novo formation of tissues, organs, and other structures was, in fact, considered to be merely an increase in size. By contrast, proponents of epigenesis, beginning with Aristotle, held that the fertilized egg was, in fact, formless and unorganized and that ontogeny was actually a process whereby organs arose de novo by a gradual differentiation of the unorganized egg into an adult organism. Since virtually all of the pertinent facts of cell biology and embryology were unknown prior to 1700, both camps were forced to take refuge in metaphysics and theology for a “full” explanation of ontogeny.

Gradually, however, as more and more information became available increasingly sophisticated theories replaced the older views, although most biologists continued to align themselves with one or the other camp until late in the 19th century. It was only when the understanding of cell biology became sufficient to provide a rationale for experimentation that the problem of development was finally resolved. Thus, by 1900 a sketchy but, nonetheless, valid outline of the developmental process was available that has served as the foundation for all subsequent progress in this field.

Not surprisingly, by the opening of the 20th century a number of psychologists and biologists had shown a growing interest in how this new information in embryology and cell biology could aid their understanding of neural and behavioral development. With a few notable exceptions, however, most of them failed to fully comprehend either the facts or the significance of the theoretical resolution of the preformation-epigenesis debate. Consequently, new, albeit familiar, debates arose over similar issues, which were now couched, however, in terms of nature vs. nurture, heredity vs. environment and maturation vs. experience.

Much of this chapter is devoted to tracing the history of these debates in the late 19th and early 20th century. One of my major contentions is that if biologists and psychologists interested in neurobehavioral development had made a more concerted and sincere effort to understand the details of the earlier preformation-epigenesis controversy, as well as the facts supporting its subsequent resolution, then the resulting long and often bitter arguments over the nature vs. nurture issue would have been more quickly recognized as anachronistic and counterproductive. Only recently, more than 80 years after the fact, do a majority of workers in this field finally appear prepared to acknowledge what was almost a commonplace to most leading biologists at the turn of the century, namely, that ontogeny is the result of a preorganized code in the genes that in concert with a long series of epigenetic events, involving intra- and extraorganismic stimuli, together, gradually transforms the fertilized egg into the complex, adult animal.

to the mind capable of curiosity and wonder, the embryo is the most seductive object in nature

attributed to T. H. Morgan

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, M. B. (1980). Sergei Chetverikov, the Kol’tsov Institute and the evolutionary synthesis. In Mayer, E., and Provine, W. B., (eds.), The Evolutionary Synthesis Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp. 242–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adelmann, H. B. (1966). Marcello Malpighi and the Evolution of Embryology Vol. 2, Cornell University Press, Ithaca.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. E. (1969). T. H. Morgan and the emergence of a new American biology. Quart. Rev. Biol 44:168–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. E. (1975). Genetics, eugenics and class struggle. Genetics 79:29–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. E. (1979). Naturalists and experimentalists: The genotype and the phenotype. In Coleman, W., and Limoges, C. (eds.), Studies in the History of Biology Vol. 3, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp. 179–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariens-Kappers, C. U., (1917). Further contributions to neurobiotaxis. IX. An attempt to compare the phenomena of neurobiotaxis with other phenomena of taxis and tropism. J. Comp. Neur 27:261–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P., and Sullivan, E. V. (1970). Theory and Problems of Child Development Grune & Stratton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, K. E. von (1828, 1837). Entwicklungsgeschichte der Thiere: Beobachtung und Reflexion Bornträger, Königsberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bain, A. (1855). The Senses and the Intellect Parker and Son, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1895) Mental Development in the Child and Race Macmillan, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1897). Social and Ethical Interpretations in Mental Development: A Study in Social Psychology Macmillan, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and Evolution Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balfour, F. M. (1880). A Treatise on Comparative Embryology Macmillan, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bannister, R. C. (1979). Social Darwinism: Science and Myth in Anglo-American Thought Temple University Press, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, L. F. (1901). The Nervous System and Its Constituent Neurons Appleton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, W. (1909). Mendel’s Principles of Heredity Cambridge University Press, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, E. (1911). Human Heredity Macmillan, New York (English translation, 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Beach, F. A. (1955). The descent of instinct. Psychol Rev 62:401–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, H. (1900). Experimentell-anatomische Studien über die durch den Mangel optischer Reize veranlassten Entwicklungshemmungen im Occiptallappen des Hundes oder der Katze. Arch, für Psychiat 33:521–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergson, H. (1911) Creative Evolution Modern Library edition 1944, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, L. L. (1924). Instinct A Study in Social Psychology Holt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertalanffy, L. von (1933). Modern Theories of Development, an Introduction to Theoretical Biology Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1894). Human faculty as determined by race. Proc. Amer. Assoc. Adv. Sci 43:301–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1919). Eugenics. Sci. Monthly 3:471–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bok, S. T. (1915). Stimulogenous fibrillation as the cause of the structure of the nervous system. Psychiat et. Neurol 19:393–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, F. E. (1910). Principles of Education Scribner’s and Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E. G. (1957). A History of Experimental Psychology Appleton, Century, Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breed, F. S. (1911). The development of certain instincts and habits in chicks. Behav. Monogr 1:1–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, W. K. (1915). The Foundations of Zoology Columbia University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, J., and Weintraub, M. (1976). A history of infant intelligence testing. In M. Lewis (ed.), Origins of Intelligence: Infancy and Early Childhood Plenum, New York, pp. 19–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. D. (1981). Gene expression in eukaryotes. Science 211:667–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt, R. W. (1980). Lamarckism in Britain and the United States, in Mayr, E., and Provine, W. (eds.), The Evolutionary Synthesis Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp. 343–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmichael, L. (1925). Heredity and environment: Are they antithetical? J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol 20:245–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmichael, L. (1926). The development of behavior in vertebrates experimentally removed from the influence of external stimulation. Psychol. Rev 33:51–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carmichael, L. (1927). A further study of the development of behavior in vertebrates experimentally removed from the influence of external stimulation. Psychol. Rev 34:34–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, W. B. (1874). Principles of Mental Physiology, H. S. King London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P., and Danchin, A. (1976). Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature 264:705–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P., and Mikoshiba, K. (1978). Genetic and “epigenetic” factors regulating synapse formation in vertebrate cerebellum and neuromuscular junction. Prog. Brain Res 48:43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, A. (1977). The Legacy of Malthus Knopf, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Child, C. M. (1915). Individuality in Organisms The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Child, C. M. (1921). The Origin and Development of the Nervous System The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Child, C. M. (1924). Physiological Foundations of Behavior Holt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchill, F. B. (1968). August Weismann and a break from tradition. J. Hist. Biol 1:91–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchill, F. B. (1973). Chabry, Roux, and the experimental method in nineteenth century embryology. In Biere, R. N., and Westfall, R. S. (eds.), Foundations of Scientific Method: The Nineteenth Century Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp. 161–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. (1924). Correlated anatomical and physiological studies of the growth of the nervous system in Amphibia. III. The floor plate of Amblystoma. J. Comp. Neur 37:37–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, G. E. (1929). Anatomy and the Problem of Behavior Cambridge University, Cambridge (Hafner reprint, 1964).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. (1979). J. B. Watson: The Founder of Behaviorism Routledge and Kegan Paul, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, W. (1965). Cell, nucleus and inheritance: An historical study. Proc. Amer. Phil. Soc 109:124–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conklin, E. G. (1922). Heredity and Environment in the Development of Man Princeton University Press, Princeton. 4th edition.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, C. H. (1896). Nature versus nurture in the making of social careers. Proc. Nat. Conf. Social. Welfare 23:399–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, C. H. (1922). Human Nature and the Social Order, Charles Scribner’s Sons, New York (1st edition, 1902).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, R. S. (1977). Nature and nurture: the interplay of biology and politics in the work of Francis Galton. In Limoges, C., and Coleman, W. (eds.), Studies in the History of Biology Vol. 1, Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp. 133–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cravens, H. (1970). American scientists and the heredity-environment controversy, 1883–1940. Ph. D. Dissertation, University Of Iowa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex 2nd edition, Murray, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals Appleton, London (University of Chicago reprint, 1965).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, E. (1796). Zoonomia; or the Law of Organic Growth, Vol. 1, E. Earle, Philadephia (4th American edition, 1818).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, F. (1911). The Life and Letters of Charles Darwin Appleton, New York (Basic Books edition, 1959, Vol. 2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, C. B. (1911). Heredity in Relation to Genetics Holt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawidowicz, L. S. (1975). The War Against the Jews 1933–1945 Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, E. H. (1976). Gene Activity in Early Development Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, P. E. (1914). The Recapitulation Theory and Human Infancy Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delage, Y. (1903). La Structure du Protoplasma et Les Theories sur l’Heredite et Les Grands Problemes de la Biologie Generale 2nd edition, Reinwald, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delage, Y., and Goldsmith, M. (1912). The Theories of Evolution Huebsch, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Detwiler, S. R. (1936). Neuroembryology An Experimental Study Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, H. (1889). Intracellular Pangenesis Open Court, Chicago (third edition, 1910).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, S. (1980). Francis Galton and American psychology. In Rieber, R., and Salzinger, K. (eds.), Psychology: Theoretical and Historical Perspectives Academic, New York, pp. 44–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, H. H. (1890–1891). Anatomical observations on the brain and several senseorgans of the blind, deaf-mute, Laura Dewey Bridgeman, Amer. J. Psychol 3:293–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, H. H. (1891–1892). Anatomical observations on the brain and several sense organs of the blind, deaf-mute, Laura Dewey Bridgeman. Amer. J. Psychol 4:248–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, H. H. (1895). The Growth of the Brain: A Study of the Nervous System in Relation to Education Scott, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driesch, H. (1894). Analytische Theorie der Organischen Entwicklung Engelmann, Leipzig.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Driesch, H. (1914). The History and Theory of Vitalism Macmillan, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drummond, H. (1894). The Ascent of Man, A. Burt Company, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drummond, W. B. (1915). The Child, His Nature and Nurture Dent, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlap, K. (1919). Are there any instincts? J. Abnorm. Psychol 14:307–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L.C. (1965). A Short History of Genetics McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, H. D. (1976). Evolution, Development and Children’s Learning Goodyear, Pacific Palisades, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forrest, D. W. (1974). Francis Galton, the Life and Work of a Victorian Genius Elek, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S., 1895, Project for a scientific psychology. In Bonaparte, M., Freud, A., and Kris, E. (eds.), The Origins of Psychoanalysis, Letters to Wilhelm Fliess, Drafts and Notes: 1887–1902 Basic Books, New York, 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S., 1916, Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. In The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Vols. 15–16, Hogarth Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freidel, F. (1970). America in the Twentieth Century 3rd edition, Knopf, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromme, A. (1941). An experimental study of the factors of maturation and practice in the behavioral development of the embryo of the frog, Rana pipiens. Genet. Psychol. Monogr 24:219–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, J. L., and Thompson, W. R. (1960). Behavior Genetics Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1865). Hereditary talent and character. Macmillan s Magazine 12:157–166 and 318–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary Genius Macmillan, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1874). English Men of Science, Their Nature and Nurture 2nd edition, Macmillan, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1876). The history of twins, as a criterion of the relative powers of nature and nurture. J. Anthro. Inst 3:391–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1889). Natural Inheritance Macmillan, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gasking, E. B. (1966). Investigations Into Generation, 1651–1828 Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasman, D. (1971). The Scientific Origins of National Socialism: Social Darwinism in Ernst Haeckel and the German Monist League American Elsevier, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • George, H. (1879). Progress and Poverty Appleton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L. (1928). Infancy and Human Growth Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L. (1929). Maturation and infant behavior patterns. Psychol. Rev 36:307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L. (1946). The ontogenesis of infant behavior, In Carmichael, L. (ed.), Manual of Child Psychology Wiley, New York, pp. 294–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L., 1952, Arnold Gesell, In Boring, E. G., Langfeld, H. S., and Werner, H. (eds.), A History of Psychology in Biography Vol. 4, Clark University Press, Worcester, Mass., pp. 123–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L., 1954, The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In Carmichael, L. (ed.), Manual of Child Psychology 2nd edition, Wiley, New York, pp. 335–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L., and Gesell, B. C. (1912). The Normal Child and Primary Education Ginn & Co., Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A. L., and Thompson, H. (1934). Infant Behavior: Its Genesis and Growth McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ghiselin, M. T. (1973). Darwin and evolutionary psychology. Science 179:964–968.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, E. (1952). Rendezvous with Destiny Harper, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldschmidt, R. (1938). Physiological Genetics McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldschmidt, R. (1940). The Material Basis of Evolution Yale University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldschmidt, R. (1956). Portraits from Memory: Recollections of a Zoologist University of Washington Press, Seattle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1970). Conceptions of prenatal behavior. In Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S., and Rosenblatt, J. S. (eds.), Development and Evolution of Behavior, Essays in Memory of T. C. Schnehia Freeman, San Francisco, pp. 111–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1972). Zing-Yang Kuo: Radical scientific philosopher and innovative experimentalist (1898–1970). J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol 80:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G., 1973, Introduction to behavioral embryology, In G. Gottlieb (ed.), Behavioral Embryology Academic Press, New York, pp. 3–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G., 1976, Conceptions of prenatal development: Behavioral embryology. Psychol. Rev 83:215–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (ed.) (1978). Introduction In Early Influences Academic Press, New York, pp. 3–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phytogeny Belknap Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham-Brown, T. (1915). On the activity of the central nervous system of the unborn fetus of the cat. J. Physiol 49:208–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. H. (1962). Douglas Alexander Spalding: The first experimental behaviorist. J. Gen. Psychol 67:299–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, P. H. (1967). Spalding and his influence on research in developmental behavior. J. Hist. Behav. Sci 3:168–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, S. P. (1967). A Textbook of Physiological Psychology Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurdon, J. B. (1974). The Control of Gene Expression in Animal Development Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haeckel, E. (1876). Die Perigenesis der Plastidule oder die Wellenzeugung der Lebensth-eilchen Reimer, Berlin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haeckel, E. (1889). The History of Creation K. Paul, Trench, Trübner Co., London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haeckel, E. (1910). The Evolution of Man Vol. 1, Putnam and Sons, New York (translation of 5th German edition).

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggquist, G., 1965, Presentation speech, In Nobel Lectures, Physiology of Medicine 1922–1941 Elsevier, Amsterdam, p. 377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidane, J. S. (1931). The Philosophical Basis of Biology Doubleday, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion, and Education Vol. 1, Appleton, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, G. S., 1909, Evolution and psychology. In Fifty Years of Darwinism, Modern Aspects of Evolution Holt, New York, pp. 251–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, T. S. (1951). A Source Book in Animal Biology Hafner reprint, 1964, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haller, J. S., Jr. (1971). Outcasts from Evolution, Scientific Attitudes of Racial Inferiority, 1859–1900 University of Illinois, Urbana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haller, M. H. (1963). Eugenics: Hereditarian Attitudes in American Thought Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V., 1957, The concept of “development” in biology. In Harris, D. B. (ed.), The Concept of Development University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp. 49–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V., 1969, Hans Spemann and the organizer concept. Experientia 25:1121–1125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V. (1970). Von Baer: Man of many talents. Quart. Rev. Biol 45:173–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V. (1973). Anatomical and physiological basis of embryonic motility in birds and mammals, In Gottlieb, G. (ed.), Behavioral Embryology Academic Press, New York, pp. 52–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamburger, V. (1980). Embryology and the modern synthesis in evolutionary biology. In Mayr, E., and Provine, W. (eds.), The Evolutionary Synthesis Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, pp. 97–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway, D. J. (1976). Crystals, Fabrics, and Fields: Metaphors ofOrganicism in Twentieth-Century Developmental Biology Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harreil, W., and Harrison, R. (1938). The rise and fall of behaviorism. J. Gen. Psychol 18:367–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, W. A., 1981, Neural activity and development. Ann. Rev. Physiol 43:689–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R. G. (1904). An experimental study of the relation of the nervous system to the developing musculature of the frog. Amer. J. Anat 3:197–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R. G. (1935). On the origin and development of the nervous system studied by the methods of experimental embryology. Proc. Roy. S oc. Lond B 108:155–196. Reprinted in

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R. G. Harrison (1969). Organization and Development of the Embryo Yale University Press, New Haven, pp. 117–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, R. G. (1937). Embryology and its relations. Science 85:369–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haverkamp, L., and Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Effects of neural blockade on behavioral development in Xenopus laevis embryos. Neurosci. Abst 7:181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, C. J. H. (1941). A Generation of Materialism, 1871–1900 Harper and Row, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. (1949). Organization of Behavior Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henle, M. (1980). The influence of Gestalt psychology in America. In Rieber, R., and Salzinger, K. (eds.), Psychology: Theoretical and Historical Perspectives Academic, New York, pp. 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hering, E. (1870). On Memory and the Specific Energies of the Nervous System, Open Court, Chicago. (3rd edition, 1902).

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrick, C. J. (1924). Neurological Foundations of Animal Behavior Holt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1972). Nature as nurture: Behaviorism and the instinct doctrine. Behaviorism 1:23–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, O. (1894). The Biological Problem of Today, Preformation or Epigenesis? The Basis of the Theory of Organic Development Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higham, J. (1955). Strangers in the Land: Patterns of American Nativism, 1860–1925 Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A., and Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1973). Constraints on Learning Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch, J. (1967). Behavior—genetic, or “experimental,” analysis: The challenge of science versus the lure of technology. Amer. Psychol 22:118–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • His, W. (1974). Unsere Körperform und das physiologische Problem ihrer Entstehung Vogel, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobhouse, L. T. (1915). Mind in Evolution 2nd edition, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter, R. (1955). Social Darwinism in American Thought Beacon, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingworth, H. L. (1929). How we learn our reflexes. Psychol. Rev 35:430–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holt, E. B. (1931). Animal Drive and the Learning Process Vol. 1, Holt, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. McV. (1961). Intelligence and Experience Ronald Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, J. McV. (1979), Psychological development: Early experience. Ann. Rev. Psychol 30:103–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, T. H. (1869). Critiques and Addresses (1873) Macmillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, F. (1970). The Logic of Living Systems, A History of Heredity Lane, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. (1978). Developmental Neurobiology Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology Vol. 1, Dover, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkinson, J. W. (1909). Experimental Embryology Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, H. S. (1906). Behavior of the Lower Organisms Indiana University Press, Bloomington (1976 reprint of the original).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, H. S. (1924). Heredity and environment. Sci. Monthly 19:225–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, H. S. (1925). Prometheus; of Biology and the Advancement of Man, E. P. Dutton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, H. S. (1930). The Biological Basis of Human Nature Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johannsen, W. (1909). Elemente der exakten Erblichkeitslehre Fischer, Jena.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonich, G. (1968). The Sane Positivist: A Biography of Edward Thorndike Wesleyan University Press, Middletown, Conn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessen, W. (1965). The Child Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, B. (1902). Social Evolution Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, R. F. (1978). Epigenesis: The role of biological models in developmental psychology. Human Development 21:141–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1971). The Case of the Mid-Wife Toad Pan Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A., and Smythies, J. R. (1969). Beyond Reductionism Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kropotkin, P. A. (1902). Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution McClure, Phillips Co., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1921). Giving up instincts in psychology. J. Philo 18:645–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1922). How are our instincts acquired? Psychol. Rev 29:344–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1924). A psychology without heredity. Psychol. Rev 31:427–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1929). The net result of the anti-heredity movement in psychology. Psychol. Rev 36:181–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1932). Ontogeny of embryonic behavior in Aves. IV. The influence of embryonic movements upon the behavior after hatching. J. Comp. Psychol 14:109–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1967). The Dynamics of Behavior Development, an Epigenetic View Random House, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1970). The need for coordinated efforts in developmental studies, In Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S., and Rosenblatt, J. S. (eds.), Development and Evolution of Behavior, Essays in Memory of T. C. Schneirla Academic Press, New York, pp. 181–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, G. T., and Woodworth, R. S. (1911). Elements of Physiological Psychology Scribner’s, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamarck, J. B. (1809). Zoological Philosophy, Macmillan, London. [1963 Hafner reprint of the 1914 English translation by H. Elliott.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Lankester, E. R. (1877). Notes on the embryology and classification of the animal kindgdom. Quart. J. Microscop. Sci 17:399–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1934). Nervous mechanisms in learning. In Murchison, C. (ed.), A Handbook of General Experimental Psychology Clark Univ. Press, Worcester, Mass, pp. 456–496.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1938). Experimental analysis of instinctive behavior. Psychol. Rev 45:445–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S., and Watson, J. B. (1913). Notes on the behavior of a young monkey. J. Anim. Behav 3:114–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quart. Rev. Biol 28:237–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S., and Rosenblatt, J. S., (eds.), Development and Evolution of Behavior, Essays in Memory ofT. C. Schneirla Academic Press, New York, pp. 17–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. M. (1978). Nature, nurture, and dynamic interactionism. Human Develop 21:1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewes, G. H. (1877). The Physical Basis of Mind, Problems of Life and Mind, 2nd series, Osgood, Boston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, R. (1981). Seeds of change in embryonic development. Science 214:42–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, A. M. (1908). Evolution, Social and Organic Kerr, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillie, F. R. (1927). The gene and the ontogenetic process. Science 66:361–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipsitt, L. P. (1967). In Stevenson, W. H., Hess, E. H., and Rheingold, H. L. (eds.), Early Behavior, Comparative and Developmental Approaches Wiley, New York, pp. 225–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lockard, R. B. (1971). Reflections on the fall of comparative psychology: is there a message for us all? Amer. Psychol 26:168–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and the Modification of Behavior University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovejoy, A. O. (1936). The Great Chain of Being Harvard University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludmerer, K. M. (1969). American geneticists and the eugenics movement: 1905–1935. J. Hist. Biol 2:337–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luys, J. (1882). The Brain and Its Function Appleton, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Maier, N. R. F., and Schneirla, T. C. (1935). Principles of Animal Psychology McGraw-Hill, New York. [1964 Dover enlarged edition.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, S. A., and Detwiler, S. R. (1926). The reactions of Amblystoma embryos following prolonged treatment with chloretone. J. Exp. Zool 45:279–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maudsley, H. (1876). The Physiology of Mind Macmillan, London. McDougall, W. (1921). An Introduction to Social Psychology 14th edition, J. W. Luce, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, M. (1935). Growth: A Study of Johnny and Jimmy Appleton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, M. (1946). Maturation of behavior. In Carmichael, L. (ed.), Manual of Child Psychology Wiley, New York, pp. 332–369.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McKinnell, R. G. (1978). Cloning, Nuclear Transplantation in Amphibia University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. (1895). On the observation of abnormalities of children. The Child Study Monthly 1:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (1848). Principles of Political Economy Little & Brown, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, W. (1898). The Nature and Development of Animal Intelligence Unwin, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moltz, H. (1965). Contemporary instinct theory and the fixed action pattern. Psychol. Rev 72:27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. L. (1895). Some definitions of instinct. Natur. Sci 7:321–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. L. (1900). Animal Behavior Arnold, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1895). Half embryos and whole embryos from one of the first two blastomeres of the frog’s egg. Anat. Anzeig 10:4–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1909). What are “factors” in Mendelian explanations? Proc. Amer. Breeders Assoc 5:365–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1912). The Development of the Frog’s Egg Macmillan, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1925). Evolution and Genetics Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H. (1934). The mechanism and laws of heredity. In Murchison, C. (ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology Clark University Press, Worcester, Mass, pp. 109–154.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. H., Sturtevant, W. H., Muller, H. J., and Bridges, C. B., 1915, The Mechanism of Mendelian Heredity Holt, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mosse, G. L. (1964). The Crisis of German Ideology: Intellectual Origins of The Third Reich Grosset & Dunlap, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müeller, J. (1843). Elements of Physiology Lea & Blanchard, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Münsterberg, H. (1885). Die Lehre von der naturlichen Anpassung in ihrer Entwicklung, Anwendung und Bedeutung Metzger and Wittig, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers, C. S., 1910, Instinct and intelligence. Brit. J. Psychol 3:209–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myerson, A. (1925). The Inheritance of Mental Disease Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Needham, J. (1959). A History of Embryology 2nd edition, Abelard-Schuman, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, N. (1898). The Development of the Child Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1972). Embryology of behavior in birds: A critical review of the role of sensory stimulation in embryonic movement. In Voous, K. H. (ed.), Proceedings of the XVth Congress of Ornithology Brill, Leiden, pp. 283–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1974). The ontogeny of behavior in the chick embryo. In Lehrman, D. S., Hinde, R. A., Shaw, E., and Rosenblatt, J. S. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior Vol. 5, Academic Press, New York, pp. 133–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1975). Changing perspectives of infancy: philosophical, psychological and biological views of behavior development. N. C. J. Mentl. Hlth 7:1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1977). A review of “The Dynamics of Behavior Development” (2nd ed. 1976) by Z.-Y. Kuo. Quart. Rev. Biol 52:327–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1978). G. E. Coghill (1872–1941): Pioneer neuroembryologist and developmental psychobiologist. Perspect. Biol. Med 22:44–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1979a). Remembrance of things past: Observations on the development of behavior, then and now. Develop. Psychobiol 12:iii-v.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1979b). Laura Bridgeman’s brain: An early consideration of functional adaptations in neurogenesis. Develop. Psychobiol 12:533–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W., 1981, Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In Connolly, K., and Prechtl, M. (eds.), Maturation and Development of Behavior, J. P. Lippincott, Philadelphia, pp. 73–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1982). The neuroembryological study of behavior: progress, problems, perspectives. In Hunt, R. K. (ed.), Topics in Developmental Biology, Vol. 17, Neural Development, Academic, New York (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, J. M. (1940). The non-specificity of the germ-layers. Quart. Rev. Biol 15:1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, J. M. (1955). Problems, concepts and their history. In Willer, B. H., Weiss, P. A., and Hamburger, U. (eds.), Analysis of Development, W. B. Saunders Co., New York, pp. 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, J. M. (1967). Essays in the History of Embryology and Biology, M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Packe, M. St. John (1954). The Life of John Stuart Mill Seeker & Warburg, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastore, N. (1949). The Nature-Nurture Controversy Kings Crown, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partridge, G. E. (1912). Genetic Philosophy of Education, An Epitome of the Published Educational Writings of President G. Stanley Hall of Clark University Sturgis and Walton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, K. (1930). The Life, Letters and Labours of Francis Galton Vol. 3a, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perez, B. (1888). The First Three Years of Childhood Arno Press, New York. [1975 reprint.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Pflüger, E. (1883). Ueber den Einfluss der Schwerkraft auf die Teilung der Zellen und auf die Entwicklung des Embryo. Arch. Gesmt. Physiol. Mensch. Thiere 31:312–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and Knowledge University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1977). Behavior and Evolution Pantheon, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piatt, J. (1948). Form and casuality in neurogenesis, Biol. Rev 23:1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pillsbury, W. B. (1916). The Fundamentals of Psychology Macmillan, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Preyer, W. (1885). Specielle Physiologie des Embryo Grieben, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radi, E. (1930). The History of Biological Theories Oxford University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raikov, B.E. (1968). Karl Ernst von Baer, 1792–1876. Sein Leben und sein Werk, Acta Historica Leopoldina, J. A. Barth, Leipzig, No. 5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramony Cajal, S. (1911). Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de l’Homme et des Vertèbres Institute Ramony Cajal del S. C. I. C., Madrid. [1952–1955 reprint.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., and Lipsitt, L. P. (1970). Experimental Child Psychology Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1884). Mental Evolution in Animals Appleton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanes, G. J. (1896). The Life and Letters of George John Romanes, Longmans, Green Co., London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (1878). Ueber die Verzweigungen der Blutgefässe des Menschen. Eine morphologische Studie. Jenaische Zeitschrift für Naturwissenschaftlichen 12:205–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (1881). Der Kampf der Theile im Organismus, W. Englemann, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (1888). Contributions to the developmental mechanics of the embryo. On the artificial production of half-embryos by destruction of one of the first two blastomeres and the later development (postgeneration) of the missing half of the body. Vire how s Archiv für pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie und für klinische Medizin 114:113–153. [translation In B. H. Willier and J. M. Oppenheimer, Foundations of Experimental Embryology Prentice-Hall, New Jersey, 1964, pp. 2–37.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (1894). The problems, methods and scope of developmental mechanics (translated by W. M. Wheeler). Wood Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole 8th Lecture: 149–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (ed.) (1905). Die Entwickelungsmechanik, ein neuer Zweig der biologischen Wissenschaft. In Vortrage und Aufstaze über Entwickelungsmechanik der Organismen Englemann, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roux, W. (1923). Wilhelm Roux in Halle a.s. In Grote, R. L. (ed.), Die Medizin der Gegenwort in Selbstdarstellungen Meiner, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, E. S. (1916). Form and Function, A Contribution to the History of Animal Morphology Murray, London.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, E. S. (1930). The Interpretation of Development and Heredity, A Study in Biological Method Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russett, C. E. (1976). Darwin in America, the Intellectual Response, 1865–1912 Freeman, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmalhausen, I. I. (1949). Factors of Evolution: The Theory of Stabilizing Selection Blakiston, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, G. H. (1880). Der Thierische Wille Abel, Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneiria, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative psychology. In Harris, D. B. (ed.), The Concept of Development University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp. 78–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneiria, T. C. (1961). Instinctive behavior, maturation—experience and development. In Kaplan, B., and Wapner, S. (eds.), Perspective in Psychology Theory, Essays in Honor of Heinz Werner, International Universities Press, pp. 303–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneiria, T. C. (1966). Behavioral development and comparative psychology. Quart. Rev. Biol 41:283–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, H. M. (1899). Organic Education, Heath, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. (1975). Your ancients revisited: A history of child development. In Hetherington, E. M. (ed.), Review of Child Development Research Vol. 5, University of Chicago, Chicago, pp. 1–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P., and Hager, J. L. (1972). Biological Boundaries of Learning Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semon, R. (1906). Die Mneme als erhaltendes Prinzip im Wechsel des organischen Geschehens. [English translation of 2nd edition, 1921, Macmillan, London.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Senn, M. J. E. (1975). Insights on the child development movement in the United States. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop 40:1–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, W. S., 1899, Notes on the psychic development of the young white rat. Amer. J. Psychol 11:80–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spalding, D. A. (1872). On instinct. Nature 6:485–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spalding, D. A. (1873). Instinct, with original observations on young animals. Macmillan s Magazine 27:282–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spalding, D. A. (1875). Instinct and acquisition. Nature 12:507–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spemann, H. (1938). Embryonic Development and Induction [Hafner reprint, 1967, New York.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Spemann, H. (1943). Forschung und Leben, J. Englehorns, Stuttgart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1866). Principles of Biology Appleton, New York, 2 vols.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1880). Principles of Psychology Vol. 1, 3rd ed., Appleton, New York. [1st ed. published in 1855.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. R. (1921). William Harvey, Obstetric Physician and Gynecologist Harrison and Sons, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1965). Embryogenesis of behavioral nerve nets. In Dehaan, R. L., and Ursprung, H. (eds.), Organogenesis Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, pp. 161–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1951). Mechanisms of neural maturation. In Stevens S. S. (ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology Wiley, New York, pp. 236–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steele, E. J. (1979). Somatic Selection and Adaptive Evolution: On the Inheritance of Acquired Characteristics William and Wallace, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stout, G. F. (1915). A Manual of Psychology 3rd edition, Hinds, Noble, Eldredge, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, E. R. (1976). The Generation Before the Great War Anchor, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanzi, E. (1895). I fatti e le induzioni nell’odierna istologia del sistema nervosa. Rivista Sperimentai Freniatria Medicina Legale delle Alienazioni 19:419–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Temkin, O. (1950). German concepts of ontogeny and history around 1800. Bull. Hist. Med 24:227–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, D. W. (1942). Growth and Form 2nd edition, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913a). Educational Psychology Vol. 1, The Original Nature of Man Columbia University Press, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913b). Eugenics: with special reference to intellect and character. Pop. Sei, Monthly 83:125–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Timerman, J. (1981). Prisoner Without a Name, Cell Without a Number Knopf, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tobach, E. and Aronson, L. R. (1970). T. C. Schneirla, a biographical note. In Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S., and Rosenblatt, J. S. (eds.), Development and Evolution of Behavior, Essays in Memory ofT. C. Schneirla Freeman, San Francisco, pp. xi-xviii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C., (1924). The inheritance of maze-learning ability in rats. J. Comp. Psychol 4:1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. B. (1973). Behavioral embryology. In Carterett, E. C., and Friedman, M. P. (eds.), Handbook of Perception Vol. 3, Academic Press, New York, pp. 89–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tryon, R. C. (1940). Genetic differences in maze-learning ability in rats. 39th Yearbook National Social Studies Education, Public School Publishing, Bloomington, Illinois, pp. 111–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuchman, B. W. (1966). The Proud Tower, A Portrait of the World Before the War, 1890–1918 Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verworn, M. (1899). General Physiology Macmillan, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, C. H. (1936). How Animals Develop Norton, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Waddington, C. H. (1939). Organizers and Genes Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, L. (1883). Dynamic Sociology Appleton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, H. C. (1921). A History of the Association Psychology Scribner’s and Sons, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1903). Animal Education: An Experimental Study of the Psychical Development of the White Rat, Correlated with the Growth of the Nervous System University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1906). The need of an experimental station for the study of certain problems in animal behavior. Psychol. Bull 3:159–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1908). The Behavior of noddy and sooty terns. Carnegie Inst. Publ 2:187–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychol. Rev 20:158–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1914). Behavior, an Introduction to Comparative Psychology Holt, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1917). Practical and theoretical problems in instinct and habits. In Suggestions of Modern Science Concerning Education Macmillan, New York, pp. 51–99.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist Lippincott, Philadelphia.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1924). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist Lippincott, Philadelphia. [revision of 1919 edition.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1925). Behaviorism The Peoples Institute, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1927). The behaviorists looks at instinct. Harpers Magazine 155:228–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1928). The Ways of Behaviorism Harper, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1930). John Broadus Watson. In Murchison, C. (ed.), A His tory of Psychology in Autobiography Vol. 3, Clark University Press, Worcester, Mass., pp. 271–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., and Lashley, K. S. (1915). An historical and experimental study of homing. Carnegie Inst. Publ 7:9–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., and McDougall, W. (1929). The Battle of Behaviorism, An Exposition and an Exposure Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., and Watson, R. R. (1928). Psychological Care of Infant and Child Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, A., 1883 On heredity. In Essays Upon Heredity and Kindred Biological Problems [Collected essays first published 1889, Clarendon Press, Oxford; reprinted 1977 by Dabor Science Publications, Oceanside, New York, pp. 69–105.]

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, A. (1893). The Germ Plasm, A Theory of Heredity Scribner’s and Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, A. (1894). The Effect of External Influences Upon Development (The Romanes Lecture), Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, A. (1896). On Germinal Selection as a Source of Definite Variation Open Court, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismann, A. (1904). The Evolution Theory Vol. 1, E. Arnold, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. A. (1939). Principles of Development, A Text in Experimental Embryology [Hafner, New York, 1969 (facsimile of original).]

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. A. (1941). Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of coordination. Comp. Psychol. Monogr 17:1–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. A. (1967). When one plus one does not equal two. In Schmitt, F. O. (ed.), The Neurosciences: A Study Program Rockefeller, New York, pp. 801–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, W. R. (1922a). The value for social psychology of the concept of instinct. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol 16:334–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, W. R. (1922b). The meaning of inherited and acquired in reference to instinct. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol 17:153–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, W. R. (1923). The anti-instinct fallacy. Psychol. Rev 30:228–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wessells, N. K. (1977). Tissue Interactions and Development, W. A. Benjamin, Inc., Menlo Park, California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, W. M. (1898). Caspar Friedrich Wolff and the Theoria generationis. Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole 15th lecture:265–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, R. H.. and Perkins, F. T. (1932). Principles of Mental Development Crowell, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, C.O. (1878). The embryology of Clepsine. Quart. J. Microscop. Sei 18:215–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, C. O. (1893). The inadequacy of the cell-theory of development. Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole, 6th lecture: 105–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, C. O. (1894a). Evolution and Epigenesis. Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station. Woods Hole, 10th lecture:203–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, C. O. (1894b). Bonnet’s theory of evolution, a system of negations, Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole 11th lecture:225–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, C. O. (1894c). The Palingenesia and the germ doctrine of Bonnet. Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole 12th lecture:241–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willier, B. H., and Oppenheimer, J. M. (1967). Foundations of Experimental Embryology Prentice-Hall, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. B. (1893). The mosaic theory of development. Woods Hole Biological Lectures, Marine Biological Station, Woods Hole 1st lecture: 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. B. (1896). The Cell in Development and Inheritance, 1st edition, Macmillan, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. B. (1900). The Cell in Development and Heredity, 2nd revised edition, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. B. (1925). The Cell in Development and Heredity, 3rd revised edition, Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S. (1978). Synchronies in mental development: An epigenetic perspective. Science 202:939–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, C. F. (1759). Theoria Generationis Halle (as cited in J. Oppenheimer, 1967).

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1921). Psychology: A Study of Mental Life Holt, New York.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Woltereck, R. (1909). Weitere experimentelle Untersuchungen über Artveränderung, speziell über das Wesen quantitativer Artunterschiede bei Daphniden. Verhandlungeni deutsch zoologische Gesamt schaff 19:110–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, R. M. (1970). Mind, Brain and Adaptation in the Nineteenth Century Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zigler, E. (1963). Metatheoretical issues in developmental psychology. In Marx, M. H. (ed.), Theories in Contemporary Psychology Macmillan, New York, pp. 341–369.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1982 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Oppenheim, R.W. (1982). Preformation and Epigenesis in the Origins of the Nervous System and Behavior: Issues, Concepts, and their History. In: Bateson, P.P.G., Klopfer, P.H. (eds) Ontogeny. Perspectives in Ethology, vol 5. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7578-8_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-7578-8_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4615-7580-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-7578-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics